From silvia.luraghi at unipv.it Wed Sep 3 15:58:06 2014 From: silvia.luraghi at unipv.it (Silvia Luraghi) Date: Wed, 3 Sep 2014 17:58:06 +0200 Subject: Reminder: possible workshop at for ICHL22, 27-31 July 2015, Naples Message-ID: Space in diachrony we would like to call your attention on our proposal for a workshop at ICHL 22 in Naples (27-31 July 2015). The workshop hasn't been accepted yet, but for the proposal we need a list of possible titles. We invite to send us a title by September 10th if you are interested. If our proposal is accepted, you will have to send an abstract following the instructions in the ICHL web page: www.ichl22.unina.it Send your reactions to: Silvia Luraghi > Tatiana Nikitina > Important dates: Deadline for workshop proposals: 20 September 2014 Notification of acceptance of workshop proposals: 20 October 2014 Deadline for submission of abstracts for general sessions and workshops: 30 January 2015 Notification of acceptance of papers for general sessions and workshops: 30 March 2015 Workshop proposal for ICHL22, 27-31 July 2015, Naples Spatial relations in diachrony Convenors: Silvia Luraghi, University of Pavia Tatiana Nikitina, CNRS Chiara Zanchi, University of Pavia The workshop addresses changes in the coding of spatial relations, with a focus on the coding of similarities and differences among spatial relations, or among variants of the same spatial relation. Topics that we would like to discuss include the source-goal asymmetry, differential marking of spatial relations, polysemy or lack of polysemy among markers of spatial relations, and the related diachronic developments. Asymmetries between goals and sources Recent research has demonstrated a number of differences in the encoding of goals and sources of motion. In general, goals of motion are expressed more frequently and in more fine-grained ways than sources (Stefanowitsch & Rohde 2004; Regier & Zheng 2007, inter alia). The asymmetry also shows up in more subtle syntactic phenomena: unlike sources, which often behave as adjuncts, goals tend to share properties with verbal arguments, and they are also more likely than sources to be incorporated in the argument structure of verbal applicatives (Baker 1988; Filip 2003). Patterns of polysemy within systems of spatial marking also point in the same direction: static locations are commonly coded by the same markers as goals of motion, and in a way distinct from sources (Blake 1977, Noonan 2009, Nikitina 2009, Pantcheva 2010, Zwarts 2010). Not that this pattern of polysemy means that diachronic mergers of source and location are not attested; much to the contrary, many individual locative markers in European languages – such as French dedans ‘inside’ or Ancient Greek ópisthe(n) ‘behind’ – often go back to ablative expressions, suggesting an earlier ablative-locative transfer (Mackenzie 1978 with examples from the Indo-European language phylum, Israeli Hebrew, and two Austronesian languages, Fijian and Sonsorol-Tobi). What seems clear from the evidence adduced by Mackenzie, as well as from other instances of the semantic extension described above (cf., for example, Bennett 1989, Nikitina & Spano 2014, Luraghi 2009 and 2010a), is that once a marker acquires the locative meaning, it loses the original ablative meaning. Thus, while the extension from source to location is attested, possibly even more frequently than commonly believed, polysemy tends to be avoided. Note, however, that special types of landmarks (spatial referents, human beings) often allow some overlap in the use of ablative and locative encoding, and can be at the origin of ablative-locative transfers (Eckhoff, Thomason, de Swart 2013, Luraghi 2009 and 2014). What accounts for the difference between the observed synchronic patterns of spatial encoding, which tend to conflate static locations and goals, and the frequently attested individual instances of ablative-locative syncretism? How do ablative-locative transfers come about? How do different types of goal-source asymmetry develop historically? Differential marking of landmarks The encoding of certain spatial relations depends on the type of landmark, and non-conventional landmarks (e.g. human beings) often require special types of encoding (Luraghi 2011). With time, such differential marking may give rise to markers that are no longer obviously related to the original spatial concept. For example, the dative has been argued to be “genetically nothing else than an offshoot of the locative used with personal nouns” (Kurylowicz 1964: 190; cf. also Aristar 1996). How do patterns of differential marking of landmarks develop and what are their conditioning factors? What types of spatial relation are more likely to produce such asymmetries? What is the possible relation between the comitative, which implies the simultaneous involvement of two entities (often human beings) in a single event, and the locative, which implies physical coincidence or at least proximity? Diachronically, comitative markers seem to arise from markers of static location; however, synchronic locative-comitative polysemy seems to be avoided, just like the locative-ablative polysemy. More research is needed on the diachronic relation between spatial and comitative markers, as at present, most evidence comes from Indo-European languages (Stolz, Luraghi 2014). If location indeed functions as a source of comitatives cross-linguistically, what accounts for the virtual absence of synchronic polysemy between the two semantic roles? Asymmetries in the encoding of path As compared to sources and goals of motion, the role of path remains largely understudied. In the light of cross-linguistic coding tendencies, goal (allative), source/origin (ablative), and (static) location (locative) seem to be more ‘basic’ spatial relations than path. As argued in Stolz (1992: 30), there is a tendency for case marking related to spatial relations to exhibit ‘Dreigliedrigkeit’, i.e. a tripartite structure featuring dedicated coding devices for location, direction and source. Indeed, path can often be coded through cases/adpositions that usually indicate location, as in English Mary walks in the field. / The child is running in the street. How are different kinds of path encoded, and where does this encoding come from? How is the distinction between unidirectional and multidirectional paths represented in different languages, and how does it develop historically? The workshop addresses these and other issues in order to better understand the nature of asymmetries in the encoding of spatial relations and shed light on the diachronic relationship between goals, sources, paths, and static locations. List of possible participants with tentative titles References Aristar, Anthony Rodriguez. 1996. The relationship between dative and locative: Kurylowicz’s argument from a typological perspective. Diachronica 13: 207-224. Baker, Mark C. 1988. Incorporation: A theory of grammatical function changing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Bennett, David C. 1989. Ablative-locative transfers: evidence from Slovene and Serbo-Croat. Oxford Slavonic Papers 22: 133-154. Blake, Barry J. 1977. Case marking in Australian languages. No. 23 in Linguistic Series. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies. Eckhoff, Hanne Martine, Olga A. Thomason and Peter de Swart. 2013. Mapping out the Source domain. Studies in Language 37/2: 302–355. Filip, Hana. 2003. Prefixes and the delimitation of events. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 11: 55–101. Luraghi, Silvia 2009. A model for representing polysemy: The Italian preposition da. In Jacques François, Eric Gilbert, Claude Guimier, Maxi Krause, éds. Actes du Colloque “Autour de la préposition”, Caen, Presses Universitaires de Caen, 167-178. Luraghi, Silvia. 2010. Adverbial Phrases. In A New Historical Syntax of Latin, Ph. Baldi and P. Cuzzolin (eds.). Berlin/ New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 19-107. Luraghi, Silvia. 2011. Human landmarks in spatial expressions: from Latin to Romance. In S. Kittilä, K. Västi, J. Ylikoski (eds.), Case, Animacy and Semantic Roles. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 207-234. Luraghi, Silvia. 2014. Plotting diachronic semantic maps. The role of metaphor. In S. Luraghi & H. Narrog, eds., Perspectives on Semantic Roles. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 99-150. Mackenzie, J. Lachlan. 1978. Ablative-locative transfers and their relevance for the theory of case-grammar. Journal of Linguistics 14: 129-375. Nikitina, Tatiana. 2009. Subcategorization pattern and lexical meaning of motion verbs: A study of the Source/Goal ambiguity. Linguistics 47: 1113-41. Nikitina, Tatiana and Marianna Spano. 2014. 'Behind' and 'in front' in Ancient Greek: A case study in orientation asymmetry. In On Ancient Grammars of Space: Linguistic research on the expression of spatial relations and motion in ancient languages, S. Kutscher & D. Werning (eds). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 67-82. Noonan, Michael. 2009. Patterns of development, patterns of syncretism of relational morphology in the Bodic languages. In The Role of Semantics and Pragmatics in the Development of Case, J. Barðdal and S. Celliah (eds.). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins, 261-282. Pantcheva, Marina. 2010. The syntactic structure of Locations, Goals, and Sources. Linguistics 48/5: 1043-1081. Regier, Terry & Mingyu Zheng. 2007. Attention to endpoints: A cross-linguistic constraint on spatial meaning. Cognitive Science 31: 705–719. Stefanowitsch, Anatol & Ada Rohde. 2004. The goal bias in the encoding of motion events. In Studies in Linguistic Motivation, G. Radden & K.-U.Panther (eds.). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 249-268. Stolz, Thomas, Cornelia Stroh and Aina Urdze. 2006. On comitatives and Related Categories. A Typological Study with Special Focus on the Languages of Europe. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Stolz, Thomas. 1992. Lokalkasussysteme. Wilhelmsfeld: Gottfried Egert Verlag. Zwarts, Joost. 2010. A hierarchy of locations: Evidence from the encoding of direction in adpositions and cases. Linguistics 48: 983-1009. -- Silvia Luraghi Università di Pavia Dipartimento di Studi Umanistici, Sezione di Linguistica Teorica e Applicata Strada Nuova 65 I-27100 Pavia tel.: +39/0382/984685 Web page personale: http://lettere.unipv.it/diplinguistica/docenti.php?&id=68 -------------- next part -------------- An HTML attachment was scrubbed... URL: -------------- next part -------------- _______________________________________________ Histling-l mailing list Histling-l at mailman.rice.edu https://mailman.rice.edu/mailman/listinfo/histling-l From francois at vjf.cnrs.fr Mon Sep 15 13:54:11 2014 From: francois at vjf.cnrs.fr (Alex Francois) Date: Mon, 15 Sep 2014 15:54:11 +0200 Subject: ICHL22 - Workshop proposal, CfP: "Non-cladistic approaches to language genealogy" Message-ID: Dear historical linguists, We would like to draw your attention to the following workshop we are proposing to organise at the next ICHL conference. If you are interested in taking part in this event, you are welcome to contact us as early as possible, using the contact details given below (and in the attached file). Best, Alex _________ Alex François LACITO-CNRS , France Australian National University , Canberra Personal homepage __________________ Workshop proposal: “*Non-cladistic approaches to language genealogy*” *Organisers* Siva Kalyan (Australian National University): siva.kalyan at anu.edu.au Alexandre François (CNRS–LaCiTO, ANU): francois at vjf.cnrs.fr To be held at the 22nd *International Conference on Historical Linguistics* (ICHL22 ), *27–31 July 2015*, Naples. *Rationale* Ever since it was popularised by August Schleicher (1853, 1873), the family-tree model has been the dominant paradigm for representing the historical relations among the languages in a family. The tree model is not without its strengths. Not only does it represent patterns of relatedness among languages, but it also commits itself to reconstructing the sequence of historical and social events that gave rise to these patterns. Further, the formal simplicity of cladistic representations makes it possible to apply powerful computational techniques from biology, which have become increasingly popular in recent years (Ringe et al. 2002, Gray et al. 2010, among many others). However, the advantages of the tree model come at the cost of making a very restrictive assumption: namely, that language families evolve primarily by splitting, with subsequent loss of contact. Put another way, the tree model assumes that once two speech varieties have started to diverge, it is no longer possible for innovations to diffuse from one to the other. This assumption excludes the possibility of overlapping subgroups. Yet it is well-known that in a dialect network, innovations can diffuse between speech varieties that have already diverged from each other (Bloomfield 1933). Furthermore, many language families are known to have arisen through the gradual breakup of a dialect network. In such language families—which Ross (1988: 8) calls *linkages*—innovations diffuse in intersecting patterns, leading naturally to the formation of overlapping subgroups. Under a tree model, such intersecting innovations would be explained away as instances of “(horizontal) contact”, to be contrasted with "genealogical change"; but this is unhelpful, as it fails to recognise that the same process of diffusion underlies all instances of language change, whether "genealogical" or "contact-induced" (Croft 2000). In a linkage, the assumptions of the tree model are not satisfied, and any cladistic representation is necessarily inadequate (Heggarty et al. 2010, François 2014, Kalyan & François forthcoming). There is thus a great need for rigorous studies of linkages that follow the essential principles of the Comparative Method, yet do not presuppose the validity of cladistic approaches. Such work is indeed possible, and is capable of yielding insights about linguistic and social history; this has already been shown in several case studies of specific language families, such as Geraghty (1983) on Fijian, Ross (1988) on Western Oceanic, and Toulmin (2009) on the Kamta branch of Indo-Aryan, Magidow (2013) on Arabic dialects, among many others. We also need to develop new ways of representing language genealogy, which dispense with the shortcomings of the tree model, while preserving its strengths. Many proposals have been made in the literature (e.g. "tree envelopes": Southworth 1964; isogloss maps: Anttila 1972; "*truncated octopus*-like trees": Hock 1991; NeighborNet: Bryant et al. 2005; trees with "contact edges": Nakhleh et al. 2005; "glottometric diagrams": Kalyan & François forthcoming), yet so far there is no widely accepted, satisfactory solution. More discussion would be welcome among historical linguists, to achieve consensus on the best methodology (or methodologies) to represent linguistic subgroups when they intersect. Finally, any proposed non-cladistic representation of genealogy will raise the question of how to interpret it in historical terms. Addressing this requires theoretical discussion of the social conditions and mechanisms of language change that lead to the development of a linkage (see Ross 1997, François 2011, 2014). Non-cladistic approaches to language genealogy are still in their infancy; we hope that this workshop serves to spur further research into this hitherto-neglected, but fundamentally important area. *Scope of the workshop* This workshop welcomes presentations which provide one or more of the following: 1. Data from a language family that poses problems for the family-tree model—in particular, a language family that exhibits extensive internal contact. 2. Application of existing non-cladistic computational methods (e.g. NeighborNet, multidimensional scaling, Structure [Pritchard et al. 2000], Historical Glottometry) to an existing dataset. 3. Innovative proposals for modeling or representing language genealogy in a non-cladistic way. 4. Discussion (either theoretical or with respect to a particular language family) of the social conditions and mechanisms of language change that are responsible for linkage-like vs. tree-like genealogies. *Presenters *(provisional list) · Paul Geraghty, “The origins of Proto Polynesian: Tokalau Fijian revisited” · Paul Heggarty, “Splits, shared innovations…and the real world. How methodological dogmas distort our ‘histories’ of languages and their speakers.” · Alexander Magidow, TBA · (other presenters, TBA) Prospective participants are invited to contact Alexandre François ( francois at vjf.cnrs.fr) and Siva Kalyan (sivakalyan.princeton at gmail.com) to express their interest. A provisional title would be welcome. *Important dates* Deadline for workshop proposals: 20 September 2014 Notification of acceptance of workshop proposals: 20 October 2014 Deadline for submission of abstracts for general sessions and workshops: 30 January 2015 Notification of acceptance of papers for general sessions and workshops: 30 March 2015 *References* Anttila, Raimo. 1972. *An introduction to historical and comparative linguistics*. New York: Macmillan. Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. *Language*. New York: Holt. Bryant, David, Flavia Filimon and Russell D. Gray. 2005. Untangling our past: Languages, Trees, Splits and Networks. In R. Mace, C. Holden & S. Shennan (eds.), *The Evolution of Cultural Diversity: Phylogenetic Approaches*. London: UCL Press. 69–85. [link ] Croft, William A. 2000. *Explaining language change: an evolutionary approach* (Longman Linguistics Library). Harlow, England: Longman. François, Alexandre. 2011. Social ecology and language history in the northern Vanuatu linkage: A tale of divergence and convergence. *Journal of Historical Linguistics *1(2):175–246. [link ] François, Alexandre. 2014. Trees, waves and linkages: models of language diversification. In Claire Bowern & Bethwyn Evans (eds.), *The Routledge handbook of historical linguistics*, 161–189. New York: Routledge. [link ] Geraghty, Paul A. 1983. *The history of the Fijian languages* (Oceanic Linguistics Special Publication No. 19). Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. Gray, Russell D., David Bryant & Simon J. Greenhill. 2010. On the shape and fabric of human history. *Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B* 365(1559). 3923–3933. [link ] Heggarty, Paul, Warren Maguire & April McMahon. 2010. Splits or waves? Trees or webs? How divergence measures and network analysis can unravel language histories. *Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B* 365(1559). 3829–3843. [link ] Hock, Hans Henrich. 1991. *Principles of Historical Linguistics*. 2nd edition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Kalyan, Siva & Alexandre François. Forthcoming. Freeing the Comparative Method from the tree model: A framework for Historical Glottometry. In Ritsuko Kikusawa & Lawrence A. Reid (eds.), *Let’s Talk about Trees: Tackling problems in representing phylogenic relationships among languages* (Senri Ethnological Studies). Ōsaka: National Museum of Ethnology. [link ] Magidow, Alexander. 2013. *Towards a Sociohistorical Reconstruction of Pre-Islamic Arabic Dialect Diversity*. PhD dissertation. Middle Eastern Studies, University of Texas, Austin. 492 pp. [link ] Nakhleh, Luay, Don Ringe & Tandy Warnow. 2005. Perfect phylogenetic networks: a new methodology for reconstructing the evolutionary history of natural languages. *Language* 81(2). 382–420. [link ] Pritchard, Jonathan K., Matthew Stephens and Peter Donnelly. 2000. Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data. *Genetics* 155. 945–959. [link ] Ringe, Don, Tandy Warnow & Ann Taylor. 2002. Indo-European and computational cladistics. *Transactions of the Philological Society* 100(1). 59–129. Ross, Malcolm D. 1988. *Proto Oceanic and the Austronesian languages of Western Melanesia* (Pacific Linguistics, Series C – 98). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. Ross, Malcolm D. 1997. Social networks and kinds of speech-community event. In Roger Blench & Matthew Spriggs (eds.), *Archaeology and language 1: theoretical and methodological orientations*, 209–261. London: Routledge. Schleicher, August. 1853. Die ersten Spaltungen des indogermanischen Urvolkes. In Johann Gustav Droysen & G. W. Nitzsch (eds.), *Allgemeine Monatsschrift für Wissenschaft und Literatur*, 786–787. Braunschweig: C. A. Schwestchke & Sohn. Schleicher, August. 1873. *Die Darwinsche Theorie und die Sprachwissenschaft: Offenes Sendschreiben an Herrn Dr. Ernst Häckel, o. Professor der Zoologie und Director des zoologischen Museums an der Universität Jena*. 2nd edition. Weimar: Hermann Böhlau. Southworth, Franklin C. 1964. Family-tree diagrams. *Language* 40(4). 557–565. Toulmin, Matthew. 2009. *From linguistic to sociolinguistic reconstruction: the Kamta historical subgroup of Indo-Aryan* (Pacific Linguistics 604). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. -------------- next part -------------- An HTML attachment was scrubbed... URL: -------------- next part -------------- A non-text attachment was scrubbed... Name: ICHL22_Workshop-proposal_Francois-Kalyan.pdf Type: application/pdf Size: 985023 bytes Desc: not available URL: -------------- next part -------------- _______________________________________________ Histling-l mailing list Histling-l at mailman.rice.edu https://mailman.rice.edu/mailman/listinfo/histling-l From silvia.luraghi at unipv.it Wed Sep 3 15:58:06 2014 From: silvia.luraghi at unipv.it (Silvia Luraghi) Date: Wed, 3 Sep 2014 17:58:06 +0200 Subject: Reminder: possible workshop at for ICHL22, 27-31 July 2015, Naples Message-ID: Space in diachrony we would like to call your attention on our proposal for a workshop at ICHL 22 in Naples (27-31 July 2015). The workshop hasn't been accepted yet, but for the proposal we need a list of possible titles. We invite to send us a title by September 10th if you are interested. If our proposal is accepted, you will have to send an abstract following the instructions in the ICHL web page: www.ichl22.unina.it Send your reactions to: Silvia Luraghi > Tatiana Nikitina > Important dates: Deadline for workshop proposals: 20 September 2014 Notification of acceptance of workshop proposals: 20 October 2014 Deadline for submission of abstracts for general sessions and workshops: 30 January 2015 Notification of acceptance of papers for general sessions and workshops: 30 March 2015 Workshop proposal for ICHL22, 27-31 July 2015, Naples Spatial relations in diachrony Convenors: Silvia Luraghi, University of Pavia Tatiana Nikitina, CNRS Chiara Zanchi, University of Pavia The workshop addresses changes in the coding of spatial relations, with a focus on the coding of similarities and differences among spatial relations, or among variants of the same spatial relation. Topics that we would like to discuss include the source-goal asymmetry, differential marking of spatial relations, polysemy or lack of polysemy among markers of spatial relations, and the related diachronic developments. Asymmetries between goals and sources Recent research has demonstrated a number of differences in the encoding of goals and sources of motion. In general, goals of motion are expressed more frequently and in more fine-grained ways than sources (Stefanowitsch & Rohde 2004; Regier & Zheng 2007, inter alia). The asymmetry also shows up in more subtle syntactic phenomena: unlike sources, which often behave as adjuncts, goals tend to share properties with verbal arguments, and they are also more likely than sources to be incorporated in the argument structure of verbal applicatives (Baker 1988; Filip 2003). Patterns of polysemy within systems of spatial marking also point in the same direction: static locations are commonly coded by the same markers as goals of motion, and in a way distinct from sources (Blake 1977, Noonan 2009, Nikitina 2009, Pantcheva 2010, Zwarts 2010). Not that this pattern of polysemy means that diachronic mergers of source and location are not attested; much to the contrary, many individual locative markers in European languages ? such as French dedans ?inside? or Ancient Greek ?pisthe(n) ?behind? ? often go back to ablative expressions, suggesting an earlier ablative-locative transfer (Mackenzie 1978 with examples from the Indo-European language phylum, Israeli Hebrew, and two Austronesian languages, Fijian and Sonsorol-Tobi). What seems clear from the evidence adduced by Mackenzie, as well as from other instances of the semantic extension described above (cf., for example, Bennett 1989, Nikitina & Spano 2014, Luraghi 2009 and 2010a), is that once a marker acquires the locative meaning, it loses the original ablative meaning. Thus, while the extension from source to location is attested, possibly even more frequently than commonly believed, polysemy tends to be avoided. Note, however, that special types of landmarks (spatial referents, human beings) often allow some overlap in the use of ablative and locative encoding, and can be at the origin of ablative-locative transfers (Eckhoff, Thomason, de Swart 2013, Luraghi 2009 and 2014). What accounts for the difference between the observed synchronic patterns of spatial encoding, which tend to conflate static locations and goals, and the frequently attested individual instances of ablative-locative syncretism? How do ablative-locative transfers come about? How do different types of goal-source asymmetry develop historically? Differential marking of landmarks The encoding of certain spatial relations depends on the type of landmark, and non-conventional landmarks (e.g. human beings) often require special types of encoding (Luraghi 2011). With time, such differential marking may give rise to markers that are no longer obviously related to the original spatial concept. For example, the dative has been argued to be ?genetically nothing else than an offshoot of the locative used with personal nouns? (Kurylowicz 1964: 190; cf. also Aristar 1996). How do patterns of differential marking of landmarks develop and what are their conditioning factors? What types of spatial relation are more likely to produce such asymmetries? What is the possible relation between the comitative, which implies the simultaneous involvement of two entities (often human beings) in a single event, and the locative, which implies physical coincidence or at least proximity? Diachronically, comitative markers seem to arise from markers of static location; however, synchronic locative-comitative polysemy seems to be avoided, just like the locative-ablative polysemy. More research is needed on the diachronic relation between spatial and comitative markers, as at present, most evidence comes from Indo-European languages (Stolz, Luraghi 2014). If location indeed functions as a source of comitatives cross-linguistically, what accounts for the virtual absence of synchronic polysemy between the two semantic roles? Asymmetries in the encoding of path As compared to sources and goals of motion, the role of path remains largely understudied. In the light of cross-linguistic coding tendencies, goal (allative), source/origin (ablative), and (static) location (locative) seem to be more ?basic? spatial relations than path. As argued in Stolz (1992: 30), there is a tendency for case marking related to spatial relations to exhibit ?Dreigliedrigkeit?, i.e. a tripartite structure featuring dedicated coding devices for location, direction and source. Indeed, path can often be coded through cases/adpositions that usually indicate location, as in English Mary walks in the field. / The child is running in the street. How are different kinds of path encoded, and where does this encoding come from? How is the distinction between unidirectional and multidirectional paths represented in different languages, and how does it develop historically? The workshop addresses these and other issues in order to better understand the nature of asymmetries in the encoding of spatial relations and shed light on the diachronic relationship between goals, sources, paths, and static locations. List of possible participants with tentative titles References Aristar, Anthony Rodriguez. 1996. The relationship between dative and locative: Kurylowicz?s argument from a typological perspective. Diachronica 13: 207-224. Baker, Mark C. 1988. Incorporation: A theory of grammatical function changing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Bennett, David C. 1989. Ablative-locative transfers: evidence from Slovene and Serbo-Croat. Oxford Slavonic Papers 22: 133-154. Blake, Barry J. 1977. Case marking in Australian languages. No. 23 in Linguistic Series. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies. Eckhoff, Hanne Martine, Olga A. Thomason and Peter de Swart. 2013. Mapping out the Source domain. Studies in Language 37/2: 302?355. Filip, Hana. 2003. Prefixes and the delimitation of events. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 11: 55?101. Luraghi, Silvia 2009. A model for representing polysemy: The Italian preposition da. In Jacques Fran?ois, Eric Gilbert, Claude Guimier, Maxi Krause, ?ds. Actes du Colloque ?Autour de la pr?position?, Caen, Presses Universitaires de Caen, 167-178. Luraghi, Silvia. 2010. Adverbial Phrases. In A New Historical Syntax of Latin, Ph. Baldi and P. Cuzzolin (eds.). Berlin/ New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 19-107. Luraghi, Silvia. 2011. Human landmarks in spatial expressions: from Latin to Romance. In S. Kittil?, K. V?sti, J. Ylikoski (eds.), Case, Animacy and Semantic Roles. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 207-234. Luraghi, Silvia. 2014. Plotting diachronic semantic maps. The role of metaphor. In S. Luraghi & H. Narrog, eds., Perspectives on Semantic Roles. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 99-150. Mackenzie, J. Lachlan. 1978. Ablative-locative transfers and their relevance for the theory of case-grammar. Journal of Linguistics 14: 129-375. Nikitina, Tatiana. 2009. Subcategorization pattern and lexical meaning of motion verbs: A study of the Source/Goal ambiguity. Linguistics 47: 1113-41. Nikitina, Tatiana and Marianna Spano. 2014. 'Behind' and 'in front' in Ancient Greek: A case study in orientation asymmetry. In On Ancient Grammars of Space: Linguistic research on the expression of spatial relations and motion in ancient languages, S. Kutscher & D. Werning (eds). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 67-82. Noonan, Michael. 2009. Patterns of development, patterns of syncretism of relational morphology in the Bodic languages. In The Role of Semantics and Pragmatics in the Development of Case, J. Bar?dal and S. Celliah (eds.). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins, 261-282. Pantcheva, Marina. 2010. The syntactic structure of Locations, Goals, and Sources. Linguistics 48/5: 1043-1081. Regier, Terry & Mingyu Zheng. 2007. Attention to endpoints: A cross-linguistic constraint on spatial meaning. Cognitive Science 31: 705?719. Stefanowitsch, Anatol & Ada Rohde. 2004. The goal bias in the encoding of motion events. In Studies in Linguistic Motivation, G. Radden & K.-U.Panther (eds.). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 249-268. Stolz, Thomas, Cornelia Stroh and Aina Urdze. 2006. On comitatives and Related Categories. A Typological Study with Special Focus on the Languages of Europe. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Stolz, Thomas. 1992. Lokalkasussysteme. Wilhelmsfeld: Gottfried Egert Verlag. Zwarts, Joost. 2010. A hierarchy of locations: Evidence from the encoding of direction in adpositions and cases. Linguistics 48: 983-1009. -- Silvia Luraghi Universit? di Pavia Dipartimento di Studi Umanistici, Sezione di Linguistica Teorica e Applicata Strada Nuova 65 I-27100 Pavia tel.: +39/0382/984685 Web page personale: http://lettere.unipv.it/diplinguistica/docenti.php?&id=68 -------------- next part -------------- An HTML attachment was scrubbed... URL: -------------- next part -------------- _______________________________________________ Histling-l mailing list Histling-l at mailman.rice.edu https://mailman.rice.edu/mailman/listinfo/histling-l From francois at vjf.cnrs.fr Mon Sep 15 13:54:11 2014 From: francois at vjf.cnrs.fr (Alex Francois) Date: Mon, 15 Sep 2014 15:54:11 +0200 Subject: ICHL22 - Workshop proposal, CfP: "Non-cladistic approaches to language genealogy" Message-ID: Dear historical linguists, We would like to draw your attention to the following workshop we are proposing to organise at the next ICHL conference. If you are interested in taking part in this event, you are welcome to contact us as early as possible, using the contact details given below (and in the attached file). Best, Alex _________ Alex Fran?ois LACITO-CNRS , France Australian National University , Canberra Personal homepage __________________ Workshop proposal: ?*Non-cladistic approaches to language genealogy*? *Organisers* Siva Kalyan (Australian National University): siva.kalyan at anu.edu.au Alexandre Fran?ois (CNRS?LaCiTO, ANU): francois at vjf.cnrs.fr To be held at the 22nd *International Conference on Historical Linguistics* (ICHL22 ), *27?31 July 2015*, Naples. *Rationale* Ever since it was popularised by August Schleicher (1853, 1873), the family-tree model has been the dominant paradigm for representing the historical relations among the languages in a family. The tree model is not without its strengths. Not only does it represent patterns of relatedness among languages, but it also commits itself to reconstructing the sequence of historical and social events that gave rise to these patterns. Further, the formal simplicity of cladistic representations makes it possible to apply powerful computational techniques from biology, which have become increasingly popular in recent years (Ringe et al. 2002, Gray et al. 2010, among many others). However, the advantages of the tree model come at the cost of making a very restrictive assumption: namely, that language families evolve primarily by splitting, with subsequent loss of contact. Put another way, the tree model assumes that once two speech varieties have started to diverge, it is no longer possible for innovations to diffuse from one to the other. This assumption excludes the possibility of overlapping subgroups. Yet it is well-known that in a dialect network, innovations can diffuse between speech varieties that have already diverged from each other (Bloomfield 1933). Furthermore, many language families are known to have arisen through the gradual breakup of a dialect network. In such language families?which Ross (1988: 8) calls *linkages*?innovations diffuse in intersecting patterns, leading naturally to the formation of overlapping subgroups. Under a tree model, such intersecting innovations would be explained away as instances of ?(horizontal) contact?, to be contrasted with "genealogical change"; but this is unhelpful, as it fails to recognise that the same process of diffusion underlies all instances of language change, whether "genealogical" or "contact-induced" (Croft 2000). In a linkage, the assumptions of the tree model are not satisfied, and any cladistic representation is necessarily inadequate (Heggarty et al. 2010, Fran?ois 2014, Kalyan & Fran?ois forthcoming). There is thus a great need for rigorous studies of linkages that follow the essential principles of the Comparative Method, yet do not presuppose the validity of cladistic approaches. Such work is indeed possible, and is capable of yielding insights about linguistic and social history; this has already been shown in several case studies of specific language families, such as Geraghty (1983) on Fijian, Ross (1988) on Western Oceanic, and Toulmin (2009) on the Kamta branch of Indo-Aryan, Magidow (2013) on Arabic dialects, among many others. We also need to develop new ways of representing language genealogy, which dispense with the shortcomings of the tree model, while preserving its strengths. Many proposals have been made in the literature (e.g. "tree envelopes": Southworth 1964; isogloss maps: Anttila 1972; "*truncated octopus*-like trees": Hock 1991; NeighborNet: Bryant et al. 2005; trees with "contact edges": Nakhleh et al. 2005; "glottometric diagrams": Kalyan & Fran?ois forthcoming), yet so far there is no widely accepted, satisfactory solution. More discussion would be welcome among historical linguists, to achieve consensus on the best methodology (or methodologies) to represent linguistic subgroups when they intersect. Finally, any proposed non-cladistic representation of genealogy will raise the question of how to interpret it in historical terms. Addressing this requires theoretical discussion of the social conditions and mechanisms of language change that lead to the development of a linkage (see Ross 1997, Fran?ois 2011, 2014). Non-cladistic approaches to language genealogy are still in their infancy; we hope that this workshop serves to spur further research into this hitherto-neglected, but fundamentally important area. *Scope of the workshop* This workshop welcomes presentations which provide one or more of the following: 1. Data from a language family that poses problems for the family-tree model?in particular, a language family that exhibits extensive internal contact. 2. Application of existing non-cladistic computational methods (e.g. NeighborNet, multidimensional scaling, Structure [Pritchard et al. 2000], Historical Glottometry) to an existing dataset. 3. Innovative proposals for modeling or representing language genealogy in a non-cladistic way. 4. Discussion (either theoretical or with respect to a particular language family) of the social conditions and mechanisms of language change that are responsible for linkage-like vs. tree-like genealogies. *Presenters *(provisional list) ? Paul Geraghty, ?The origins of Proto Polynesian: Tokalau Fijian revisited? ? Paul Heggarty, ?Splits, shared innovations?and the real world. How methodological dogmas distort our ?histories? of languages and their speakers.? ? Alexander Magidow, TBA ? (other presenters, TBA) Prospective participants are invited to contact Alexandre Fran?ois ( francois at vjf.cnrs.fr) and Siva Kalyan (sivakalyan.princeton at gmail.com) to express their interest. A provisional title would be welcome. *Important dates* Deadline for workshop proposals: 20 September 2014 Notification of acceptance of workshop proposals: 20 October 2014 Deadline for submission of abstracts for general sessions and workshops: 30 January 2015 Notification of acceptance of papers for general sessions and workshops: 30 March 2015 *References* Anttila, Raimo. 1972. *An introduction to historical and comparative linguistics*. New York: Macmillan. Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. *Language*. New York: Holt. Bryant, David, Flavia Filimon and Russell D. Gray. 2005. Untangling our past: Languages, Trees, Splits and Networks. In R. Mace, C. Holden & S. Shennan (eds.), *The Evolution of Cultural Diversity: Phylogenetic Approaches*. London: UCL Press. 69?85. [link ] Croft, William A. 2000. *Explaining language change: an evolutionary approach* (Longman Linguistics Library). Harlow, England: Longman. Fran?ois, Alexandre. 2011. Social ecology and language history in the northern Vanuatu linkage: A tale of divergence and convergence. *Journal of Historical Linguistics *1(2):175?246. [link ] Fran?ois, Alexandre. 2014. Trees, waves and linkages: models of language diversification. In Claire Bowern & Bethwyn Evans (eds.), *The Routledge handbook of historical linguistics*, 161?189. New York: Routledge. [link ] Geraghty, Paul A. 1983. *The history of the Fijian languages* (Oceanic Linguistics Special Publication No. 19). Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. Gray, Russell D., David Bryant & Simon J. Greenhill. 2010. On the shape and fabric of human history. *Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B* 365(1559). 3923?3933. [link ] Heggarty, Paul, Warren Maguire & April McMahon. 2010. Splits or waves? Trees or webs? How divergence measures and network analysis can unravel language histories. *Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B* 365(1559). 3829?3843. [link ] Hock, Hans Henrich. 1991. *Principles of Historical Linguistics*. 2nd edition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Kalyan, Siva & Alexandre Fran?ois. Forthcoming. Freeing the Comparative Method from the tree model: A framework for Historical Glottometry. In Ritsuko Kikusawa & Lawrence A. Reid (eds.), *Let?s Talk about Trees: Tackling problems in representing phylogenic relationships among languages* (Senri Ethnological Studies). ?saka: National Museum of Ethnology. [link ] Magidow, Alexander. 2013. *Towards a Sociohistorical Reconstruction of Pre-Islamic Arabic Dialect Diversity*. PhD dissertation. Middle Eastern Studies, University of Texas, Austin. 492 pp. [link ] Nakhleh, Luay, Don Ringe & Tandy Warnow. 2005. Perfect phylogenetic networks: a new methodology for reconstructing the evolutionary history of natural languages. *Language* 81(2). 382?420. [link ] Pritchard, Jonathan K., Matthew Stephens and Peter Donnelly. 2000. Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data. *Genetics* 155. 945?959. [link ] Ringe, Don, Tandy Warnow & Ann Taylor. 2002. Indo-European and computational cladistics. *Transactions of the Philological Society* 100(1). 59?129. Ross, Malcolm D. 1988. *Proto Oceanic and the Austronesian languages of Western Melanesia* (Pacific Linguistics, Series C ? 98). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. Ross, Malcolm D. 1997. Social networks and kinds of speech-community event. In Roger Blench & Matthew Spriggs (eds.), *Archaeology and language 1: theoretical and methodological orientations*, 209?261. London: Routledge. Schleicher, August. 1853. Die ersten Spaltungen des indogermanischen Urvolkes. In Johann Gustav Droysen & G. W. Nitzsch (eds.), *Allgemeine Monatsschrift f?r Wissenschaft und Literatur*, 786?787. Braunschweig: C. A. Schwestchke & Sohn. Schleicher, August. 1873. *Die Darwinsche Theorie und die Sprachwissenschaft: Offenes Sendschreiben an Herrn Dr. Ernst H?ckel, o. Professor der Zoologie und Director des zoologischen Museums an der Universit?t Jena*. 2nd edition. Weimar: Hermann B?hlau. Southworth, Franklin C. 1964. Family-tree diagrams. *Language* 40(4). 557?565. Toulmin, Matthew. 2009. *From linguistic to sociolinguistic reconstruction: the Kamta historical subgroup of Indo-Aryan* (Pacific Linguistics 604). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. -------------- next part -------------- An HTML attachment was scrubbed... URL: -------------- next part -------------- A non-text attachment was scrubbed... Name: ICHL22_Workshop-proposal_Francois-Kalyan.pdf Type: application/pdf Size: 985023 bytes Desc: not available URL: -------------- next part -------------- _______________________________________________ Histling-l mailing list Histling-l at mailman.rice.edu https://mailman.rice.edu/mailman/listinfo/histling-l