Indigenous math

Mia Kalish MiaKalish at LEARNINGFORPEOPLE.US
Sat Apr 22 15:30:02 UTC 2006


This is such a good discussion. Rudy presents an example similar to the
examples raised over and over and over by Ethnomathematicians. (And I liked
the story, too, Rudy.) 


Lilly Wong Fillmore talks often of how Alaskan students in SPED classes (98%
of the students, in some schools) know astounding and sophisticated details
of kayak making and sailing, fishing, surviving on the ice floes, migration
patterns, and staying warm when wet. Of course, they didn't know anything
about jumping horses in Virginia, and this was how they got to be SPED kids
(along with the extra dollars to the school, of course). 

There is something very strange - I never really noticed it before, because
when you build software for people, it always reflects their corporate
culture - about how people seem to think that there is only one kind of
academic knowledge. Math comes only in one flavor. Botany comes in one
flavor, and so on. In fact, Math comes in as many flavors as there are
cultures, and so do botany, biology, and chemistry. Some things are common,
like eltse thingy + eltse thingy == naaki thingies. Other things, like
apples and oranges are not common, nor are ways of building kayaks.
Traditionally, kayaks have been custom built to the physical dimensions of
the person who owns it, rather than to a one-size-fits-all form of typical
manufacturing plants. 

But it seems to me that it has to be up to us, both to produce the articles
to show the world that there are more worlds, and that things are similar
but different in those worlds. We need to produce the materials and do the
research. 

:-) Mia

-----Original Message-----
From: Indigenous Languages and Technology [mailto:ILAT at LISTSERV.ARIZONA.EDU]
On Behalf Of Richard Zane Smith
Sent: Saturday, April 22, 2006 7:33 AM
To: ILAT at LISTSERV.ARIZONA.EDU
Subject: Re: [ILAT] Indigenous math

Rudy and Mia raise issues 
the public schools somehow haven't slowed down enought to consider. 
I think Western math ,like everything else is becoming so "specialized"
that today it creates its own wake of ignorance.
   Have you ever watched an Asian store cleric using an abacus?
Compare that image to our typical Walmart clerk on the computer.
Its obvious which one is actually using math
and its even more obvious when the computer fails.
Computers are excellent tools,but mass dependance upon them to 
"do our thinking" can create a very fragile culture of its own.
Some people still see indigenous cultures as merely offering spice, color
and frybread. It still hasn't dawned on the mainstream american,that  keys
to  
survival may lie within the enduring cultures it has sought to replace.
Richard



> 
> From: Rudy Troike <rtroike at EMAIL.ARIZONA.EDU>
> Date: 2006/04/22 Sat AM 04:16:21 CDT
> To: ILAT at LISTSERV.ARIZONA.EDU
> Subject: [ILAT] Indigenous math
> 
> This is in response to Mia's note on native math. While it does not
> necessarily
> involve native language in computing (though it might), it does reveal the
> importance of her point about math being culturally embedded, and 
relevant.
> 
> One of my favorite stories is from Barney Old Coyote, who told me of 
visiting
> an elementary school with a number of Crow youngsters attending. He was 
in
> a third-grade class, and the teacher was demonstrating how miserable the
> Crow students' math skills were, by showing that they could not do first-
grade
> arithmetic problems of adding apples and oranges. Barney Old Coyote 
asked the
> teacher if he could take over the class for a few minutes to try out
> something,
> and the teacher agreed. So he asked the class if anyone could compute the 
odds
> in a stick-ball game, giving them the parameters. The Crow students 
quickly
> responded to a number of these, computing the odds entirely in their 
heads
> with amazing speed. Their Anglo peer hadn't a clue as to how to do this, 
and
> were astounded at their classmates' mathematical skill, as was the
teacher,
> who had no idea that they could do this.
> 
> Culturally-embedded and relevant skills like this, not just in math, often
> exist but are not recognized by the formal educational curriculum, nor by
> teachers trained only to recognize and teach that, and hence are not 
rewarded
> nor built upon for more advanced development. Relevant here is Perry 
Gilmore's
> famous example of "Spelling Mississippi", in which she found that Black 
teen-
> age girls in Philadelphia, who were failing abysmally in spelling in
class,
> during their lunch hour were doing jump-rope in which they were regularly
> spelling out complicated words using a semi-special vocabulary for letter-
> names (e.g. s = "crooked letter"), but the teachers were totally unaware
that
> this activity was going on, and hence were not able to harness this 
knowledge
> to enhance classroom learning.
> 
> Motivation is also sometimes relevant, as when rural development workers 
in
> West Africa found that attempts to teach basic math to farmers was a total
> failure, until they hit on the fact that the farmers were regularly being
> ripped off by middlemen to whom they sold their produce, who gave them 
false
> information on the weights of their goods. Once they realized that a 
knowledge
> of numbers would enable them to protect their interests, they became 
highly
> motivated to learn.
> 
> On the other hand, people can also enjoy the simple intellectual pleasures
> of abstract math, and to say that native people can't do this is to
greatly
> underestimate them. I recall a story by someone who was teaching some 
mid-
> level abstract math to some rural Mayan speakers, and found that they
> enjoyed remaining in the classroom after school to challenge one another
> with math computation problems, which they treated as an intellectual 
game.
> 
>     Rudy Troike
>     University of Arizona
>     Department of English
> 

Richard Zane Smith
18474 S.Cayuga Rd.
Wyandotte Oklahoma
                                  74370



More information about the Ilat mailing list