13.450, Sum: Voicing-Conditioned Vowel Alternations

LINGUIST List linguist at linguistlist.org
Tue Feb 19 00:19:40 UTC 2002


LINGUIST List:  Vol-13-450. Mon Feb 18 2002. ISSN: 1068-4875.

Subject: 13.450, Sum: Voicing-Conditioned Vowel Alternations

Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Wayne State U.<aristar at linguistlist.org>
            Helen Dry, Eastern Michigan U. <hdry at linguistlist.org>
            Andrew Carnie, U. of Arizona <carnie at linguistlist.org>

Reviews (reviews at linguistlist.org):
	Simin Karimi, U. of Arizona
	Terence Langendoen, U. of Arizona

Editors (linguist at linguistlist.org):
	Karen Milligan, WSU 		Naomi Ogasawara, EMU
	James Yuells, EMU		Marie Klopfenstein, WSU
	Michael Appleby, EMU		Heather Taylor-Loring, EMU
	Ljuba Veselinova, Stockholm U.	Richard John Harvey, EMU
	Dina Kapetangianni, EMU		Renee Galvis, WSU
	Karolina Owczarzak, EMU

Software: John Remmers, E. Michigan U. <remmers at emunix.emich.edu>
          Gayathri Sriram, E. Michigan U. <gayatri at linguistlist.org>

Home Page:  http://linguistlist.org/

The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University, Wayne
State University, and donations from subscribers and publishers.



Editor for this issue: Marie Klopfenstein <marie at linguistlist.org>

=================================Directory=================================

1)
Date:  Thu, 14 Feb 2002 17:41:01 -0500 (EST)
From:  Elliott Moreton <moreton at vonneumann.cog.jhu.edu>
Subject:  Voicing-conditioned vowel alternations

-------------------------------- Message 1 -------------------------------

Date:  Thu, 14 Feb 2002 17:41:01 -0500 (EST)
From:  Elliott Moreton <moreton at vonneumann.cog.jhu.edu>
Subject:  Voicing-conditioned vowel alternations

Dear Linguists:

Thanks to everyone who replied to my query about vowel quality
alternations conditioned by the voicing specification of adjacent
consonants:  Dave Odden, Paul Johnston, Thorsten Schroter, Michael
Johnstone, Paul Boersma, Mary Paster, Remy Viredaz, Dieter Wunderlich,
Geoffrey S. Nathan, Wolfgang M. Schultze, Price Caldwell, Tobias Scheer,
and Viktor Tron.  To the examples I already had from English (Canadian
Raising/Southern Monophthongization) and Polish (morphophonemic u/o
alternation), they added some from Nilotic, Madurese, Scots, Shetlandic
Scots, Buchan Scots, Yakut, Maastricht Limburgian, and Czech.  They also
made short work of my putative diachronic German example.  A summary is
appended.

I'm interested in this because of an article by Erik Thomas in the Journal
of Phonetics in 2000, in which he raises the possibility that there is
hyperarticulation before voiceless obstruents.  I think this is right.
Voiceless obstruents are themselves hyperarticulated (review:  Maddieson &
Ladefoged _Patterns of Sounds_ 1996:95-99).  In English, mid and low
vowels become lower before voiceless (Summers 1987 JASA, Wolf 1979 JPhon,
Fujimura & Miller 1979 Phonetica), with more lowering closer to the
consonant.  Meanwhile, the high offglide of Eng. /ai/ becomes higher
(Thomas 2000 JPhon), as do the offglides of /au oi ei/ (Moreton 2001 LSA
and in prep, ask if you'd like a draft).

Since the hyperarticulation of [-voice] obstruents is widespread, I'd
expected phonologized alternations on the pattern of Canadian
Raising/Southern Monophthongization, and Polish, to turn up in more
places.  The present harvest is encouraging, but most of the
well-documented examples are from near relatives of English and Polish.
The Madurese alternation goes the "wrong" way; however, there the
consonant precedes rather than follows the vowel (and does not affect its
length), so it is probably a separate phenomenon.  Thanks again,

Best wishes,
Elliott Moreton

____________________________________________________________
NILOTIC

David Odden writes:

Not unrelated is something that Keith Denning did in his Stanford
dissertation, regarding the relation between breathy vs. modal voice and
vowel quality in Nilotic. Essentially, what he pointed out is that
perceived vowel height is related to vocal tract geometry, that the "back
tube" can be lengthened either by raising the tongue or lowering the
larynx, and that breathy vowels often also involve larynx lowering; hence
they also seem to be a bit higher.

The interesting thing about these cases, in comparison to the ones you
mentioned, is that they don't involve vowel length (i.e. voice and vowel
length are related, and length is related to height).  <end David Odden
quote>

This dissertation is a treasure trove of examples.  Denning proposes a
universal:

"In lgs in which ther eis a regular correlation between perceived vowel
height and phonation (i.e. pitch, phonation type or voicing in
consonants), greater height is associated with relative laryngeal laxness
and/or voicing." (p. 59)

____________________________________________________________
POLISH and CZECH (with an excursus on GERMAN)

Raising in Polish -- a morphophonemic alternation in
which some /o/s surface as [u] before an underlying voiced
coda consonant (Gussmann 1980 _Studies in Abstract Phonology_; Kenstowicz
1994 _Phon. in Gen. Gr._ 74-78):

        nom. sg.        m[u]d           'fashion'
        gen. pl.        m[o]da

Tobias Scheer <Tobias.Scheer at unice.fr> writes:

this alternation is indeed very common in Polish, but it is heavily
restricted by morphological and lexical parameters (the latter related to
frequency of the word). There is no synchronic activity for sure since
present-day [u] > alternates only with [o] if it comes from a former [o].
[u]s that have always been [u] do never alternate. Polish spelling notes
this difference: alternating [u] is written "o with an acute accent",
identifying its diachronic source, while regular [u] is spelt "u".

The same alternation concerns the two nasal vowels of Polish (written E
for the front, O for the back one hereafter):

dOb "oak NOMsg"  -  dEb-u "oak GENsg

etc. both the O-E and the o-u alternation are instances of the same
original process, which, alas for you, does not modify vowel quality, but
vowel quantity:

o > oo / __C+voice #

and there are two conditions, not just one: the input-o must occur before
a voiced consonant which on top of being voiced needs to be word-final.
Exactly the same alternation o-u is found in Czech, with the same
conditioning. Furthermore, Czech has the advantage of not having
eliminated vowel-length as Polish did, to the effect that the original
contrast in length is still visible (the forms hereafter are not spelling,
N=palatal nasal):

kuuN "horse NOMsg"  -  koNe "id. GENsg"  -  koN-sky "id., adjective"

etc.  much more data and illustration is available in a handout of a class
of mine which you can download at

http://www.unice.fr/dsl/tobias.htm

then go to "classes taught at Warsaw", and click on the course-handout.
Polish-Czech o-u is in section 12.13 on p.44ss  and a parallel German
alternation in strong verbs is discussed on p.45:  beissen "bite" where
<ss> is [s] derives a preterite biss where the [i] is short whereas
preisen "worship" where <s> is [z] derives a preterite pries where the
<ie> is long.

<End quote from Tobias Scheer>

The course handout mentioned above is very detailed and informative, and
could provide a whole semester's worth of problem sets.

____________________________________________________________
ENGLISH:  Canadian Raising/Southern Monophthongization

Canadian Raising / Southern Monophthongization (English)
-  a very widespread alternation in which /ai/, and sometimes
also /au/, is higher before voiceless codas than voiced ones
(Chambers 1973 CanJLing 18:113-135):

                        CR              SM

        tight           t^It            taIt
        tide            taId            ta:d

Paul Boersma <paul.boersma at hum.uva.nl> and Geoffrey S. Nathan
<geoffn at siu.edu> suggest that the alternation is not conditioned directly
by the voicing of the following consonant, but by the difference in vowel
length (which in turn depends on voicing).

____________________________________________________________
SCOTS:  Scottish Vowel Length Rule

Michael Johnstone <mjj1000 at hermes.cam.ac.uk> and Viktor Tron
<tron at coli@uni-sb.de> pointed out that the famous SVLR induces quality
alternations in /ai/.  The SVLR applies to Modern Scots reflexes of
certain Middle Scots vowels:

Middle Scots (16th cent.) /ei i: e: u: *  ui ou iu E a o/
Modern Scots              /%  e  u  u  !  @i ^u ju E a o/

(/%/ see below; /*/ = slashed o, Cardinal 10, upper mid front rounded; /@/
= schwa; /!/ = several diphthongs; see Aitken 1981 pp 132-133).  These
vowels are long

     before a morpheme boundary, or
     before a voiced fricative and a morpheme boundary, or
	before [r] and a morpheme boundary

and short elsewhere (Aitken 1981 p. 135).  The present-day reflex of MS
/ei/ undergoes a quality change like that of Canadian Raising/Southern
Monophthongization:  _price_ [pr at is], _prize_ = [pra:ez].  A slew of
references are provided by Viktor Tron:

A. J. Aitken (1981) The Scottish Vowel Length Rule In. M. Benskin and M.L.
Samuels (eds) So Meny People, Longuages and Tonges, pp 131-157. Edinburgh:
Middle English Dialect Project.

Scott Allan (1985) A note on AYE distribution. Journal of Linguistics 21.
pp 	 191--194

J. Derrick McClure (1977) Vowel duration in a Scottish accent. Journal of
the International Phonetic Association 7. pp 10-16

April M. S. McMahon (1991) Lexical Phonology and Sound Change: the case of
the   Scottish Vowel Length Rule. Journal of Linguistics 27. pp 29-53

James Myers (1999) Lexical Phonology and the Lexicon. Rutgers
Optimality Archive #330-0699

J. M. Scobbie, N. Hewlett and A. E. Turk (1999) Standard English in
Edinburgh and Glasgow: the Scottish vowel length rule revealed. University
of Edinburgh ms.

____________________________________________________________
BUCHAN SCOTS:  Height harmony blocked by [+voice] obstruents

Mary Paster <paster at socrates.Berkeley.EDU> writes:

According to Eugen Dieth's (1932) grammar of the dialect, the facts are
these: High, unaccented vowels in the second syllable undergo lowering to
a mid variant if the preceding vowel is non-high (i.e. mid or low -- so
it's not total height assimilation). The pattern holds for root-suffix
(with some suffixes being excepted for some reason), word-clitic, and,
apparently, within words. But the lowering harmony is blocked by voiced
obstruents (and a few select sequences of other consonants). This gives
rise to the (relatively) famously-cited pair [lase] 'lassie' vs. [ladi]
'laddie', where the lowering is blocked on the diminutive suffix in
'laddie' (I gather that Scots makes more use of the diminutive suffix than
we do, so Dieth's grammar has a ton of examples of it, and it's clearly a
robust pattern -- at least, it was in 1932).  <End Mary Paster quote>

Paster also mentions an article by Colleen Fitzgerald in a recent or
near-future _English Language and Linguistics_ (and has one of her own in
prep).

____________________________________________________________
SHETLANDIC SCOTS:  Raising before voiced sounds

Paul Johnston <johnstonp at wmich.edu> writes:

Voicing conditions vowel alternations in Shetlandic, and to some extent,
Orcadian Scots, though the "voiced" group may be more broken up.

Scots /a/    =   Shetlandic [a] before vl. sounds, [ae] before vd. ones As
in bat/bad
Scots /E/    =   Shetlandic [E] before vl. sounds, [e] or [ei] before vd.
ones As in bet/bed
Scots /a:/   =   Shetlandic [a:] before vl. sounds, [ae:] (North/West) or
[D:] (South--that's a low back rounded vowel) before vd. ones as in salt
(saut)/Maud

Some of the other vowels show this alternation too, but only before
obstruents; voiced sonorants behave like voiceless obstruents, or
introduce a third allophone.

The exact equivalent of the first and third rules also operate in some
types of Northumbrian English, particularly of the Coalfield and East
Central Part of the county of Northumberland.

bat  =  [bat]		bad  = [bae at d]
salt = [sa:t]		Maud = [mae at d]

The best [reference] for Orcadian/Shetlandic is probably mine: Johnston,
Paul.  1997.  "Regional Variation" in Jones, Charles (ed.) . The Edinburgh
History of the Scottish Language, pp. 433-513.  Relevant material is on
464, 485, 489.  Also 464 (for /E:/).  My data came from: Mather, James Y.
and Hans-Henning Speitel. 1986.  The Linguistic Atlas of Scotlkand, Vol.
3: Phonology.  London: Croom Helm. and observations.

The Nhb. stuff was personally observed.  I mentioned it in my Ph. D.
thesis, but that's unpublished.  However, I think Henry Warkentine's
(1964) hard-to-find thesis on Hexham, Nhb. dialect (I haven't got a full
ref.) may have it.  If you want to plow, you can get the whole
distribution by looking at relevant words in  Orton, Harold and Wilfrid H.
Halliday.  1962.  The Survey of English Dialects, Vol. I.  Basic Material:
The Six Northern Counties and the Isle of Man.  Leeds: Arnold.

Look at Community Nb 4 particularly.  <End Paul Johnston quote.>

____________________________________________________________
MADURESE:  Raising after voiced or voiced-aspirated C

David Odden <odden at ling.ohio-state.edu> called my attention to this one,
which is described in detail in

Stevens, Alan M. (1968).  Madurese Phonology and Morphology.  American
Oriental Series, #52.  New Haven:  American Oriental Society.

Stevens describes a class of "higher determinant" consonants, which he
writes as

                   Lab Den Alv Pal Vel
     voiced stops /b   d   d,  z   g /
     aspirates    /bh  dh  d,h zh  gh/

plus /w/ and most instances of /j/ (exceptions for morpheme juncture).
The four principal vowel phonemes /a i u @/ occur in two versions, raised
and lowered.  Following a higher-determinant consonant, vowels are raised.
This effect propagates through the word until stopped by a
lower-determinant consonant (anything that is neither higher-determinant,
/s/ or /j/ with a close juncture, [r], or [q]).  Vowels not raised are
lowered.

There is a relevant paper by Cohn and Lockwood (1994, not seen) in the
Working Papers of the Cornell Phonetics Lab.
____________________________________________________________
YAKUT:  Lengthening and breaking-to-diphthong before [+voice]

Remy Viredaz <remy.viredaz at bluewin.ch> pointed me to

Grammont, Maurice (1933).  Traite de phonetique.  Paris:  Librairie
Delagrave.

A paragraph on p. 187 reads:

We find in some languages a phenomenon which is in a certain sense the
opposite of the Latin phenomenon [devoicing of a stop with compensatory
lengthening of the preceding vowel, te:ctum < tegere].  In Osmanli, a
short vowel + voicless consonant remains short vowel + voiceless
consonant:

     Yakut           Osmanli
      /at/ 'horse'     /at/
      /ot/ 'plant'     /ot/
      /as-/ 'open'     /atS-/

But a long vowel + voiceless consonant becomes a short vowel + voiced
consonant:

     Yakut           Osmanli
      /a:t/ 'name'    /ad/
      /uot/ 'fire'    /od/
      /a:s/ 'starved' /adZ/
      /bu:t/ 'hip'    /bud/ 'thigh'
      /y:t/ 'milk'    /yd/
      /ky:s/ 'force'  /gydZ/

<end Grammont>

G. doesn't discuss the breaking-to-diphthong, but it seems that Yakut
underwent a change /o/->/o:/->/uo/ before originally voiced consonants.
Many more examples, though no published source, can be found in an
assignment from Bert Vaux's class at Harvard:

http://icg.harvard.edu/~sa34/assignments/asgt7.pdf

____________________________________________________________
MAASTRICHT LIMBURGIAN:  Lengthening and breaking-to-diphthong before
[+voice]

Paul Boersma <paul.boersma at hum.uva.nl> contributes a parallel breaking
case:

[W]e can consider a similar case in Maastricht Limburgian, in which high
vowels became (or become, in an abstract analysis) diphthongs
if a final schwa dropped after a voiced consonant. Thus:
   bli:2ve -> blEi1f 'stay-1SG'
   bli:2ven -> bli:2ve(n) 'stay-INF'
So we have vowel alternations within verb paradigms, if the consonant is
voiced. In nouns, we have similar examples:
   du:2ve -> dOu1f 'pigeon'
   du:2ven -> du:2ve(n) 'pigeons'
The free ride in this case is provided by the change from second to first
tone, which occurs precisely when schwa is dropped after an originally
voiced consonant (or currently voiced; the sonorants do it as well, as in
Polish). So the real conditioning factor is probably the presence of the
first tone, though it is hard to distinguish this from conditioning by
voicing, since there are no underlying or original first tones on the
vowels /i:/, /u:/, or /a:/ (which becomes [O:] under tone change or voiced
schwa drop). The Maastricht case is further complicated by the fact that
unlike in Polish, there are original consonant-final forms, which don't
change:
   bli:2f -> bli:2f 'stay-IMP.SG'
So underlyingly, there may be a voiced consonant here as well, but
historically there isn't. This is some extra evidence for the causation of
vowel change by tone. Now one would like to know the details of what
happened in Polish... [the Maastricht change also occurred if there was
*no* consonant,  as in ri:2e -> rEi1 'row' and ly:2(d)e -> loei1 'people']
<end Paul Boersma quote>


____________________________________________________________
GERMAN:  Not really

My original query asked about:

(3)  A "well-known sound change" in German in which long
vowels are shortened and lowered before voiceless consonants
(mentioned briefly in Kohler 1984 Phonetica 41:150-174):

        M[U]tter        'mother'
        Br[u:]der       'brother'

Several people wrote in to deny that this was anything of the sort:
Thorsten Schroter <thorsten.schroter at kau.se>, Paul Boersma
<paul.boersma at hum.uva.nl>, Remy Viredaz <remy.viridaz at bluewin.ch>, Dieter
Wunderlich <wdl at phil-fak.uni-duesseldorf.de>, Wolfgang M. Schultze
<W.Schultze at lrz.uni-muenchen.de>, and Tobias Scheer
<Tobias.Scheer at unice.fr>.

That this is not synchronic is shown by Wunderlich's examples:

	b[U]ddeln  	'dig'
	g[u:]ter		'good' strong masc.sg.nominative

Wolfgang Schultze connects it with the open/closed syllable distinction:

Though there seems to be a tendency to shorten long vowels before vl.
consonants [such as _V[a:]ter_ 'father' shifting towards _V[A]ter_ esp.
Rhenanian and some northern varieties], the standard formula is that there
is a correlation between

length/hight and V-final syllable ('open')
short/lowered and C-final syllable ('closed')

Hence we have

            M[U]t-ter    'mother'
            g[u:]-ter    'a good one'        etc.

            b[I]b-bern    'to tremble'
            B[i:]bern       'for the beavers'            etc.

And remember that the opposition M[U]tter / Br[u:]der is conditioned by
etymology (< *mat:r resp. *bhr:ter).  <End Wolfgang Schultze quote>

Tobias Scheer adds more examples of short vowels before voiced stops:
wabbern, blubbern, daddeln.

Paul Boersma and Tobias Scheer point out that the only certain correlation
is between vowel height on the one hand and vowel length (of whatever
origin) on the other:  [U] and [u:], never [U:] or [u].

Remy Viredaz comments:

The German example does not conform exactly to the formula you have used.
In many cases, a long vowel has remained long before a voiceless consonant
(Buch 'book', Hut 'hat', Mut 'courage', Haut 'skin', Euter 'udder'), and
even a short vowel has sometimes become long in that environment (Vater
'father'). I haven't looked through the 19th and 20th century grammars to
see if our "forefathers" have been able to see some order in all this.
However, the voiced vs. voiceless character of the vowel has certainly
played a role in many cases. MHG short i, u seem to have been lengthened
before a voiced obstruent (Glied 'member') or a sonant (viel 'much') and
not before a voiceless consonant (Schnitt 'a cut') or a consonant cluster.
<End Remy Viredaz quote>

Finally, Price Caldwell <price at mail.hinocatv.ne.jp> mentions the tendency
of high vowels to disappear entirely (or at least become devoiced) between
voiceless consonants.

Many thanks again to all contributors.

Sincerely,
Elliott Moreton

---------------------------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-13-450



More information about the LINGUIST mailing list