16.1351, Review: Morphology: Katamba (2003) -- Part 1

LINGUIST List linguist at linguistlist.org
Thu Apr 28 03:38:44 UTC 2005


LINGUIST List: Vol-16-1351. Wed Apr 27 2005. ISSN: 1068 - 4875.

Subject: 16.1351, Review: Morphology: Katamba (2003) -- Part 1

Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Wayne State U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
            Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>
 
Reviews (reviews at linguistlist.org) 
        Sheila Dooley, U of Arizona  
        Terry Langendoen, U of Arizona  

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org/

The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University, Wayne
State University, and donations from subscribers and publishers.

Editor for this issue: Naomi Ogasawara <naomi at linguistlist.org>
================================================================  

What follows is a review or discussion note contributed to our 
Book Discussion Forum. We expect discussions to be informal and 
interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially 
invited to join in. If you are interested in leading a book 
discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available 
for review." Then contact Sheila Dooley at collberg at linguistlist.org. 

===========================Directory==============================  

1)
Date: 26-Apr-2005
From: Kalyanamalini Sahoo < kalyanamalini at yahoo.com >
Subject: Morphology: Critical Concepts in Linguistics 

	
-------------------------Message 1 ---------------------------------- 
Date: Wed, 27 Apr 2005 23:32:10
From: Kalyanamalini Sahoo < kalyanamalini at yahoo.com >
Subject: Morphology: Critical Concepts in Linguistics 
 

EDITOR: Katamba, Francis X.
TITLE: Morphology
SUBTITLE: Critical Concepts in Linguistics
SERIES: Critical Concepts in Linguistics
YEAR: 2003
PUBLISHER: Routledge (Taylor and Francis)
ISBN: 0415270782 (for the six-volume set)
Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/15/15-1129.html


Kalyanamalini Sahoo, language consultant 

[This review divided into two parts. This part contains the Overview 
and Synopsis of Volumes I-III. The second part in a subsequent issue 
contains the Synopsis of Volumes IV-VI, the Critical Evaluation and 
References. -- Eds.]

OVERVIEW

This set of 6 volumes is a collection of 78 reprinted articles from 
various journals and books.  The anthology of articles beginning from 
1939 to the year 2000 has been carefully planned and nicely 
coordinated by the editor between the various contributors.  Volume 
1 "Word Structure: a variety of views" is constituted of 18 papers, a 
preface by the editor, and a general introduction to all the volumes in 
the set. Is it words or morphemes that are the basic units of 
morphological analysis? - has been discussed in many articles of 
volume I.  Volume II "Morphology: Primes, Phenomena and Processes" 
is constituted of 13 papers.  Articles in this Volume survey 
morphological phenomena in a huge number of genetically and 
typologically different languages.  The interaction of morphology with 
other levels of linguistics (phonology, syntax and semantics) has been 
dealt with in volumes III, IV and V. Volume III "Morphology: its relation 
to Phonology", Volume IV "Morphology: its relation to Syntax", and 
Volume V "Morphology: its relation to Semantics and the Lexicon" are 
constituted of 10 papers each.  Volume VI "Morphology: its place in 
the wider context" deals with articles which discuss morphology 
emerging in an interdisciplinary context or application-oriented field 
(e.g. psycholinguistics, computational linguistics and historical 
linguistics).  This Volume is constituted of 17 papers, and an index of 
linguistic terms referred to in all the volumes of the set. 

Three articles (one in each of the volumes II, III and VI) are in French, 
while all the other articles are in English.  Each article has its own 
Notes and References.  There is no special effort to put all the articles 
in the same format, they have just been reproduced from the 
original/source.

SYNOPSIS

VOLUME I -- WORD STRUCTURE: A VARIETY OF VIEWS

Volume I presents a variety of approaches to word-
structure.  'Whether words or morphemes are the basic unit of 
morphological description' is the topic of discussion in many articles. 
Chapter 2, 3, 4, and 13 defend a morpheme-based approach, while 
ch. 5, 7, 8, 17, 18 argue for a word-based approach. Ch.16 takes a 
radical view and denies the existence of words. Overall, this volume 
presents a unique collection of early influential studies in word 
structure analysis by leading scholars in the field. Not only that the 
authors have highlighted what phenomena a morpheme-based 
approach or a word-based approach will be able to tackle, but also in 
many articles, the authors have preferred to take a rather critical 
stand to many key notions of this theoretical approach.

This volume starts with Roman Jakobson's article 'Quest for the 
essence of language' (source: Diogenes 51 (1966): 21-37).  It surveys 
the development of theories of the linguistic sign from Ancient Greece 
to the twentieth century.  Beginning with Bloomfield's 1933 manual 
regarding the study of language with respect to 'sound' and 'meaning', 
Jakobson discusses Saussure's interpretation of the sign - 'significant' 
and 'signifié'; St. Augustine's Latinized term 'signum' comprising 
both 'signans' and 'signatum'; Charles Sanders Peirce's interpretation 
of 'sign' as 'semiosis' in terms of 'icon', 'index' and 'symbol'; 
Greenberg's grammatical universals and near-universals; Bolinger's 
importance of cross influences between sound and meaning. He 
discusses the nature of the word and the place of morphology in the 
study of language.

In chapter 2, Zellig S. Harris's article 'Morpheme alternants in linguistic 
analysis' (Source: Language 18 (1942): 169-180) focuses on 
morpheme-based approach to morphology.  It presents regular 
phonology, morphophonemics, sandhi, morphological processes like 
vowel change, morpholexical variation, suppletion etc. as cases of a 
single linguistic relation - morpheme alternants.  It proposes 
techniques for determining the morphemes of a language, gives an 
account of key concepts and analytical techniques of morpheme 
analysis in American structural linguistics.

In chapter 3, Eugene A. Nida's article 'The identification of 
morphemes' (source: Language 24 (1948): 414-441) also focuses on 
morpheme-based approach to morphology. This is in response to 
Hockett's (cf. next chapter), and Bloch's (Language 23: 399-418) 
morphemic analysis.  He treats some of the specific problems raised 
by Bloch's and Hockett's papers, which constitutes a background for 
the development of certain principles for the analysis and classification 
of morphemes.  Adopting Bloomfield's definition, he provides sub-
morphemic distinctions of phonetic form as well as of semantic value.  
According to it, a definition which contrasts one phonetic-semantic 
entity with all other entities in the language still permits the sub-
morphemic distinctions of phonetic form and semantic areas within the 
basic distinctiveness which sets off such a form from other possibly 
related forms.  Providing examples from different languages, he 
discusses 13 principles which govern the identification of morphemes.  
He classifies morphemes based on (i) the types of phonemes which 
comprise the morphemes, (ii) the positional relationship of the parts of 
the morphemes, and (iii) the positional relationship of the morphemes 
to other morphemes.

In chapter 4 'Problems of morphemic analysis' (Source: Language 23 
(1947): 321-343), Charles F. Hockett provides a critical evaluation of 
the theory of morphemic analysis as propounded by Harris (cf.chapter 
2).

Charles E. Bazell in chapter 5 'On the problem of the morpheme' 
(Source: Archivum Linguisticum 1 (1949): 1-15.) stands his ground 
and gives a good explanation of the morphemic analysis.  He tries to 
remove any skepticism about this approach by posing a more critical 
questioning of the fundamental assumptions and analytical techniques 
of morpheme theory. 

In chapter 6, Hockett's article (source: Word 10 (1954): 210-234) 
discusses two models of grammatical description: Item and Process 
(IP) and Item and Arrangement (IA).  Besides, he makes passing 
mention of the traditional Word and Paradigm (WP) approach 
employed since classical antiquity to describe the morphology of 
Sanskrit, Greek and Latin as well as many modern languages but 
which had not found favour with modern linguists in the first part of the 
twentieth century.  According to IP model, morphological description 
involves identifying morphological items and the processes which they 
undergo, while IA model essentially talks of things and the 
arrangements in which those things occur. In both the types, the 
minimal grammatical item is the morpheme, and this is the fundamental 
unit for all subsequent grammatical analysis.  However, neither 
approach turns out to be entirely satisfactory.  The paper concludes 
with a proposal for a set of five criteria for evaluating models of 
grammatical description namely, generality, specificity, inclusiveness, 
productivity and efficiency.  Judged using these criteria, none of the 
models is found to be completely satisfactory, although Hockett leans 
towards IA. However, he is concerned about its inability to deal 
effectively with a number of phenomena (e.g. internal vowel change 
as in take - took).

Chapter 7 'In defence of WP' (Source: Transactions of the Philological 
Society (1959): 116-144) is written by R. H. Robins as a response to 
the apologetic passing mention of WP by Hockett (ch.6).  Robins's 
paper presents a defence of a word-and-paradigm approach and 
shows how revamped WP can be reinstated as a model suitable for 
aspects of grammatical analysis, in particular the description of 
languages with inflecting morphology.  The distinctive characteristics 
of a WP model is: the word is taken as the basic unit of both syntax 
and morphology, and variable words are grouped into paradigms for 
the statement of their morphological forms and the listing of their 
various syntactic functions.  WP has advantages over Hockett's IA or 
IP in the sense that (i) the word as a unity is more easily susceptible to 
grammatical statement than is the individual bound morpheme; (ii) WP 
avoids some of the difficulties in morphophonology 
(morphophonemics), in the relating of grammatical structuring, which 
beset IA and to a lesser extent IP.  However, WP has its 
disadvantages also: (i) with its use of Process terminology, WP, like IP, 
implies a historic perspective and confuses synchronic linguistics with 
diachronic linguistics. (ii) WP appears to be less tidy and economical 
in requiring both Process and Arrangement as separate terms (in 
morphological and syntactic description respectively) than either IA or 
IP with their exclusive use of one or the other. (iii) a descriptive model 
is intrinsically less desirable if it makes a non-minimal element, the 
word, basic in the hierarchy of structures at the same general level of 
analysis, as against both the other models that make the minimal 
grammatical element, the morpheme, also the basic element of 
structure.  However, Robins admits that because of the immense 
complexity of language, none of the three models, IA, IP and WP, has 
worked out to be equally suitable for every part of a grammatical 
system in every language.   

In chapter 8 'Some concepts in word-and-paradigm morphology' 
(Source: Foundations of Language 1 (1965): 268-289.), Peter H. 
Matthews takes up the baton and develops an explicit account of WP 
articulating, in particular, the notion of "paradigmatic structure". 

In chapter 9 (Source: Journal of Linguistics 21 (1985): 321 -337.), 
Wolfgang U. Dressler elaborates a claim for the predictive 
(explanatory) power of Natural Morphology (NM). The first part of NM 
is a theory of universals; the second is of morphological typology; the 
third is of language specific system adequacy.  Although it does not 
have a sociolinguistic or psycholinguistic theory of its own to account 
for norms and performances, however, it consistently refers to such 
theories.

In chapter 10 (Source: Foundations of Language 16 (1982): 181-
198.), Dieter Kastovsky outlines a functional approach to word-
formation.  He concedes that it is impossible to formulate a set of 
general rules that account for all word-formation patterns in a 
language.  The dual functions of naming and syntactic 
recategorization affect word-formation in different ways, resulting in 
different outcomes depending on which of these functions is dominant.

Chapter 11 'Prolegomena to a theory of word formation' (Source: 
Linguistic Inquiry 4 (1973): 3-16.) is a study in morphology in early 
generative grammar.  In this, Morris Halle proposes that morphology 
consists of three distinct components: a list of morphemes, rules of 
word formation, and a filter containing the idiosyncratic properties of 
words.  The list of morphemes and the rules of word formation 
together define the potential words of the language.  The set of actual 
words is obtained from that of the potential words by applying to the 
latter the modifications indicated in the filter.  Morphology, thus, 
producing a long list of words designated as dictionary. Lexical 
insertion transformations select items from the dictionary and enter 
these in appropriate slots in structures representing the underlying 
constituent structure of particular sentences.  It is to these underlying 
representations that the syntactic transformations apply and generate 
the surface structure.  Contra Bresnan (1971, 1972), he does not 
accept any phonological rule applying as part of the transformational 
cycle of syntax.

Stephen R. Anderson's article 'Where's morphology?' (Source: 
Linguistic Inquiry 13 (4) (1982): 571-612.) is also a study in 
morphology in early generative grammar.  In chapter 12, Anderson 
argues that in the domain "inflection", there is a nontrivial intersection 
between the theories of syntax and morphology. In order to capture 
both the relation of inflectional morphology to the syntax, and the 
exclusion of derivational morphology from "syntactic accessibility", he 
proposes a model of morphological operations, and provides the 
outlines of a theory of "Extended Word and Paradigm" morphology as 
an account of the mechanism of inflectional specification. He proposes 
principles governing the interaction of morphological rules, and also 
notes their interaction with "phonological" rules.

In chapter 13, 'A general theory of word structure' (Source: E. 
O.Selkirk, The Syntax of Words, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1982, 
pp. 1-12.) Elisabeth O. Selkirk argues that word structure has the 
same general formal properties as syntactic structure. In generative 
grammar, as word-formation was considered as a part of syntactic 
transformations (Lees 1960), she argues that words, like sentences, 
have internal constituent structure, and they should be treated 
syntactically using phrase structure grammars.

In chapter 14, Stephen R. Anderson asks, "How much structure do 
words have?" (Source: S. R.Anderson, A-Morphous Morphology, 
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992, pp.256-291.). He 
denies any internal morphological structure for words and argues for 
a-morphous morphology in which word-internal structure is eliminated 
and replaced with conditions on the structure of the derivation of a 
word.

In chapter 15 'Autolexical syntax: a proposal for the treatment of noun 
incorporation and similar phenomena' (Source: Natural Language and 
Linguistic Theory 3 (1985): 379-439.), Jerrold M. Sadock puts forward 
a proposal for autolexical syntax, a theory in which morphology and 
syntax operate in a completely autonomous fashion and are held 
together by universal principles relating possible pairings of analyses 
sanctioned by each.  One distinct advantage of an autolexical 
treatment of a polysynthetic language is that it allows a cross-
componential analysis of phenomena such as noun incorporation 
while not requiring the mingling of syntax and morphology where there 
is no evidence for it.

In chapter 16, 'Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection' 
(Source: Kenneth Hale and S. Jay Keyser (eds), The View from 
Building 20, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1993, pp.111-176.), Morris 
Halle and Alec Marantz take a radical view and deny the existence of 
words, the lexical component and of lexical insertion. They propose a 
theory "Distributed morphology" (DM) (which adopts the basic 
organization of a principles and parameters grammar) and assume 
that it is only after all syntactic operations are completed that 
phonological expressions get inserted in a process called spell-out.  
The theory assumes underspecification of vocabulary items such that 
the phonological information of a word is not listed together with its 
matching morphosyntactic properties.  The theory also assumes that 
syntactic and morphological objects have the same types of 
constituent structures.  In particular, Distributed Morphology attempts 
to make precise the claim that all derivation of complex objects is 
syntactic. In respect to the interface between syntax and morphology, 
this architecture has a clear consequence: since the only mode of 
combination in the grammar is syntactic, it follows that in the default 
case, morphological structure simply is syntactic structure.  Moreover, 
this is a comprehensive rejection of the Lexicalist Hypothesis which 
claims that words and word-formation belong in the lexicon (see ch. 
11, 12 and 25).

Taking a different view, in chapter 17, Chet Creider and Richard 
Hudson elaborate an account of inflectional morphology in Word 
Grammar (Source: Lingua 107 (1999): 163-187.).  In their approach 
the relevance of the word and the lexicon is accepted.  They use 
ideas from the Word-and-Paradigm tradition - Lexeme, Stem and 
Inflection - in combination with the logic of default inheritance.  They 
apply this theory to a range of different morphological data and 
compare the theory with the other contemporary approaches like a-
morphous morphology, distributed morphology and network 
morphology.

In chapter 18 'On the separation of derivation from morphology: 
toward a lexeme/morpheme-based morphology' (Source: Quaderni di 
Semantica 9(1) (1988): 3-59.), Robert Beard presents another word-
based approach.   A key feature of this Lexeme/morpheme-based 
model is the Separation Hypothesis, which assumes that the 
derivation of meaning and the realization of phonological marking are 
distinct processes in word-formation.   The function (grammatically 
relevant <<meaning>>) of derivations is not predictable on the basis of 
affixation nor vice versa.

VOLUME II -- MORPHOLOGY: PRIMES, PHENOMENA AND 
PROCESSES

The main concern of this volume is the nature of morphological 
primes, phenomena and processes. Beginning from defining a word, 
morphological phenomena like difference between inflectional and 
derivational morphology is discussed.  4 articles concerning issues in 
inflectional morphology, and a series of articles on derivational 
morphology dealing with issues in compounding, reduplication, clitics 
and particles etc. are the main topic of discussion.

In chapter 19, 'Qu'est-ce qu'un mot?' (Source: Knud Togeby, Travaux 
du Cercle Linguistique de Copenhague, Volume 5: Recherches 
Structurales, Copenhague: Nordisk Sprog-og Kulturforlag, 1949, pp. 
97-111) Knud Togeby asks the question 'what is a word?'  He 
grapples with the challenge of defining the word and identifying a set 
of robust criteria for word recognition, a theme also taken up in 
chapter 20 by John Lyons.

John Lyons in chapter 20 'The word' (Source: Introduction to 
theoretical linguistics (1968) pp 194- 208, Cambridge: Cambridge 
University Press.) gives a brief account of the primary units of 
grammatical analysis and the way in which the terms applying to them 
have been defined in modern linguistics.  (For him, the relationship 
between sentences, clauses, phrases, words and morphemes can be 
expressed as a unit of 'higher' rank being composed out of units 
of 'lower' rank.) Next, morphological phenomena are discussed.  Traditionally,
the two major divisions of morphology are said to be inflection and derivation.  
What is the difference between the two?  

In chapter 21 'Inflection' (Source: M. Hammond & M. Noonan (eds.) 
Theoretical Morphology: Approaches to Modern Linguistics, 
California: Academic Press, 1988, pp. 23- 44.), Stephen R. Anderson 
explains the properties of inflection, emphasizing its syntactically 
driven nature.  Assuming that inflection is outside of derivation, he 
claims that material introduced by inflectional rule (not lexically) on the 
basis of properties assigned in the syntax to the morphosyntactic 
representation of the word presupposes, but is not presupposed by, 
material that is present in the lexical form.  In particular, nonregular 
(hence lexical) morphology as well as material which is introduced not 
in response to the requirements of the syntax but for semantic or 
purely derivational reasons, may appear in derivational forms or 
compounds because it is in the lexicon.

Chapter 22 'Stems in Latin verbal morphology' (Source: Mark Aronoff, 
Morphology by itself: Stems and Inflectional Classes. Linguistic Inquiry 
Monograph Twenty-Two, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1994, pp. 31-
59.) stays with the same theme.  Focusing on the role of stems in 
Latin verbal inflectional morphology, Mark Aronoff examines the role 
of stems from a functional perspective in a word-based theory of 
morphology (cf. chs. 7, 10, 16, 17, 18). 

Chapter 23 'Paradigm economy' (Source: Journal of Linguistics 19 
(1983): 115-128.) treats another aspect of inflection: paradigms (cf. 
ch. 8).  Andrew Carstairs discusses how, in any inflected language, 
the inflectional resources available in some word-class or part of 
speech are distributed among members of that word-class. He argues 
that inflectional class systems depend crucially on the distinction 
between suffixal inflection and stem alternations: stem alternations 
and affixal alternation interact in ways that favour certain patterns and 
disfavour or completely exclude others.  

In chapter 24 'On rules of referral' (Source: Language 69(2) (1993): 
449-479.), Gregory Stump puts forward a proposal (in another word-
based approach) for a formal theory of rules of referral to account for 
syncretism in inflectional morphology.  This theory affords a precise 
account of a range of rule interactions involving rules of referral.  It 
furnishes a simple explanation for the fact that syncretisms donot 
always encompass whole words, for the fact that some referrals are 
bidirectional, and for the fact that two or more referrals may participate 
successively or simultaneously in the definition of a single instance of 
syncretism.

Then there follows a group of articles on derivational morphology. 

In chapter 25 "Remarks on Nominalization" (Source: R. Jacobs & P. 
Rosenbaum (eds.) Readings in English Transformational Grammar, 
Waltham, MA: Ginn, 1970, pp. 184-221.), Noam Chomsky argues in 
favour of the Lexicalist Hypothesis which hypothesizes that syntactic 
operations only apply to syntactic constituents and hence cannot be 
employed in derivational morphology.  Focusing on the contrast 
between derived nominals (which are created by derivational 
morphology in the lexicon), and -ing gerundives (which are the result 
of syntactic inflection), he claims that the former are similar to 
underived words and the latter are comparable to syntactic phrases.  
Word formation is firmly put in the lexicon; words are treated as 
syntactic atoms whose internal structure is unavailable to syntax. 

The next two articles deal with compounding.

In chapter 26 'On the creation and use of English compound nouns' 
(Source: Language 55 (1977): 810-842.), Pamela Downing deals with 
the semantic properties of noun + noun compounding in English.  She 
illustrates that the constraints on English N+N compounds cannot be 
characterized in terms of absolute limitations on the semantic or 
syntactic structures from which they are derived.  Rather, the data 
examined here reflect tendencies for compounds to be based on 
permanent, non-predictable relationships of varying semantic types, 
depending on the nature of the entity being denoted.  

Rochelle Lieber's article (ch. 27) 'Argument linking and compounds in 
English' (Source: Linguistic Inquiry 14(2) (1983): 251-286.) proposes 
an analysis of compounds with a Lexicalist framework.  The Argument-
linking Principle allows us to formulate a set of predictions about 
possible and impossible combinations of stems within each compound 
type and the productivity of the compounds in the language.  She 
argues that a compound type containing an argument-taking stem will 
never be as productive as compound types containing no argument-
taking stems.  Synthetic compounds are analyzed by referring to the 
theta grid of the verb (cf. ch. 13).

In chapter 28 'Nimboran position class morphology' (Source: Natural 
Language and Linguistic Theory 11 (1993): 559-624.), a different kind 
of phenomenon, positional class morphology, is introduced. Sharon 
Inkelas explores the Papuan language Nimboran where verbal 
morphemes are inflexibly ordered and morphemes with the same 
ordering properties are in complementary distribution (cf. ch 42).  He 
labels these verbal morphemes "positional class morphemes" and 
exemplifies new support for the theory of level-ordering.  Examining a 
large corpus of data, he offers a formal theory of position class 
morphology.

In chapter 29 'Reduplicative constructions' (Source: Joseph 
Greenberg (ed.), Universals of Human Language, Vol. 3: Word 
Structure, Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1978, pp. 297-334.) 
Edith A. Moravcsik surveys the form, meaning and distribution of 
reduplicative constructions within and across languages. She comes 
to the conclusion that:
i) phonological properties determining which part of a string be 
reduplicated in cases of partial reduplication are restricted 
to 'canonical form'-type properties like consonantality, vowelhood, and 
linear precedence among the segments and boundaries.
ii) languages usually use reduplicative patterns - i.e. quantitative form 
differentiation - for the expression of meanings that have something to 
do with the quantity of referents.
iii) reduplicative constructions express a more specific meaning than 
their unreduplicated counterparts. 

This is followed by chapter 30, which deals with metathesis.

In chapter 30 'Metathesis as a grammatical device' (Source: 
International Journal of American Linguistics 35(3) (1969): 213-219.), 
Laurence C. Thompson and M. Terry Thompson discuss metathesis in 
the Austronesian language Rotuman and the Straits Salish language 
Clallam. 

The final chapter in this volume ends with chapter 31 'Clitics and 
particles' (Source: Language 61(2) (1985): 283-305.). In this, Arnold 
M. Zwicky investigates typological properties of clitics and particles, 
phenomena whose existence complicates decisions about the status 
of the word.  He distinguishes clitics from inflectional affixes and 
independent words, and for this purpose he makes use of various 
tests like phonological tests, syntactic tests, accentual test etc..  He 
shows that in many cases items labeled 'particles' have been treated 
as clitics.  As most of the 'particles' in the literature are simply words, 
he argues that treating words with idiosyncratic distributions as 
acategorial 'particles' is wrong. Finally, he depicts a class of discourse 
markers: a grammatical category of items which are often classified 
as 'particles' but which turn out to be independent words rather than 
clitics of any sort. 

VOLUME III -- MORPHOLOGY: ITS RELATION TO PHONOLOGY

This volume focuses on the relation of morphology to phonology.  
Contra the doctrine of separation of levels, in which sound structure 
used to be described before grammatical structure, hence separating 
grammatical information when performing phonemic analysis, a series 
of articles in this volume pioneer the idea that morphological and 
phonological analysis can be intertwined in a linguistic description.

Chapter 32 is Leonard Bloomfield's classic paper on 'Menomini 
morphophonemics' (Source: Travaux du circle linguistique de Prague 
8 (1939): 105-115.), in which he demonstrates that some phonological 
alternations require a morphological trigger.  Simple words and the 
members of compounds in Menomini can be analyzed into 
morphologic elements which vary greatly in different combinations.  He 
distinguishes these morphophonemic alternations from morpholexical 
variations and describes the internal sandhi or morphophonemics of 
the language.  He sets up each morphological element in a theoretical 
basic form, and then states the deviations from this basic form which 
appear when the element is combined with other elements.

In chapter 33 'A problem in phonological alternation' (Source: 
Language 15 (1939): 1-10.) also, Morris Swadesh and CF. Voegelin 
discuss that phonological alternations are motivated by morphological 
factors (also see ch 6 for the discussion of IP).  They consider 
Tübatulabal, a Uto-Aztecan language of California, as a striking 
illustration to account for irregular or "non-patent" phonological 
alternations.

Two decades later, writing on phonotactics in early generative 
grammar, Morris Halle discusses the limitations which the language 
places on the occurrence of distinctive feature complexes in the 
sequence.  In chapter 34 'Sequential constraints' (Source: Morris 
Halle, The Sound Pattern of Russian, The Hague: Mouton, 1959, pp. 
55-75.), he has shown that there are phonological regularities 
governing the phonological realization of morphemes.  Phonological 
representations contain a minimum of specified features and the 
automatic distribution of features is governed by the following three 
types of rule:
i) the morpheme structure rules deal exclusively with the feature 
composition of individual morphemes.
ii) the morphological rules, which are part of transformational level, 
require reference not only to the feature composition of morphemes 
but also to the morpheme class to which the latter belong.
iii) the phonological rules assign features on the basis of purely 
phonological criteria; they require reference only to features and 
phonological boundaries.

Halle's idea was further articulated by Richard Stanley in 'Redundancy 
rules in phonology' (Source: Language 43(1) (1967): 393-436.), in his 
proposal for a theory of lexical redundancy (ch 35).  How should the 
linguist deal with those phonological properties of words and 
morphemes that are entirely predictable?  That is the question that 
Stanley set out to answer.  He gives particular attention to the formal 
nature of morpheme structure rules and to the use of blanks in 
representing redundancies.  However, later he replaces morpheme 
structure rules with a new formal device called 'morpheme structure 
conditions' and provides strong motivation for the preference of the 
latter one.

The fact that certain morphological rules have a close affinity with 
phonological rules goes back to the proposal by (Whitney 1889, 
Bloomfield 1933).  Such interaction between morphology and 
phonology got articulated by Paul Kiparsky as the theory of Lexical 
morphology and phonology (chapter 36) (Source: The Linguistic 
Society of Korea (ed.), Linguistics in the Morning Calm, Seoul: 
Hanshin Publishing, 1982, pp. 3-91.).  Kiparsky proposes that in this 
theory, word-formation takes place in the lexicon.  The lexicon is 
stratified on the basis of the properties of various affixes.  
Morphological rules and phonological rules belonging to the same 
stratum apply in tandem.

Kiparsky's approach, although insightful it was, had its own 
drawbacks, many of them stemming from the fact that affix-driven 
stratification all too often did not yield reliable results.  Hence, Heinz J. 
Giegerich in chapter 37 'Principles of base-driven stratification' 
(Source: Heinz J Giegerich, Lexical Strata in English, Cambridge: 
Cambridge University Press, 1999, pp. 53-99.) mounts a rescue for 
lexical phonology and morphology in the form of a revised theory with 
base-driven rather than affix-driven lexical stratification.  This model 
predicts that the 'Continuity of Strata Hypothesis' must be true on both 
the phonological and the morphological side.

In chapter 38 'A prosodic theory of nonconcatenative morphology' 
(Source: Linguistic Inquiry 12(3) (1981): 373-418.), following Harris's 
(1941, 1951) notion of long components, John McCarthy  provides a 
more successful approach to the description of nonconcatenative 
morphology which employs CV skeletal tier positions as templates for 
canonical morphological forms. This is an extension to morphology of 
Goldsmith's autosegmental phonology model. McCarthy justifies this 
theory by an analysis of the formal properties of the system of verbal 
derivation and aspect and voice inflection in Classical Arabic.  He also 
discusses reduplication and the extension of this treatment to non-
Semitic languages. 

In chapter 39 'Théorie de l'apophonie et organization des schèmes en 
sémitique' (Source: Jacqueline Lecarme, Jean Lowenstamm and Ur 
Shlonsky (eds), Research in Afroasiatic Grammar, Papers from the 
Third conference on Afroasiatic Languages, Sophia Antipolis, 1996, 
Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 2000, pp. 263-299.), Philippe Ségéral 
explores the regularities that lie beneath the surface in the apophony 
exhibited as part of the nonconcatenative morphology of Semitic 
languages.  In an analysis inspired in part by government phonology 
and in part by autosegmental phonology he shows that there exist 
phonological regularities in vowel alternations across tense-aspect 
paradigms in Akkadian.

In chapter 40 'Re reduplication' (Source: Linguistic Inquiry 13(3) 
(1982): 435-482.), Alec Marantz presents an account of reduplication 
that is based on McCarthy's proposals for template morphology.  He 
claims that reduplication is best analyzed as the affixation of a skeletal 
morpheme to a stem.  Such an analysis explains the otherwise 
puzzling interaction of reduplication with certain phonological 
processes.  The fact that reduplication processes can generally be 
characterized by a fixed consonant-vowel shape, a fact captured in 
the identification of reduplicating morphemes as C-V skeletal, provides 
considerable support for McCarthy's autosegmental representation of 
words on different tiers including a phonemic melody and a C-V 
skeleton.  Given that reduplication is simply affixation, this article also 
supports Halle's (1979) interpretation of the phonological cycle and 
Lieber's (1980) morpholexical theory dealing with the interaction of 
reduplication and phonological processes. 

In chapter 41, 'Faithfulness and identity in prosodic 
morphology'(Source: Rene Kager, Harry van der Hulst and Wim 
Zonneveld (eds), The Prosody- Morphology Interface, Cambridge: 
Cambridge University Press, 1999, pp. 218-309.), focusing on 
reduplication, infixation, root-pattern morphology and constraints on 
the canonical shape (e.g. of the minimal word) McCarthy and Prince 
use Optimality Theory to argue for a Prosodic Morphology approach, 
which claims that morphological templates are defined in terms of 
authentic units of the prosodic hierarchy (e.g. syllable, foot).

[Part 2 of this review appears in a subsequent issue

see the Editors' note at the beginning of this Part.] 

ABOUT THE REVIEWER

Kalyanamalini Sahoo has extensively worked on morphosyntactic 
investigations in the South-Asian language Oriya, including 
applicational fields like computational morphology. She received her 
Ph.D. from Norwegian University of Science & Technology, 
Trondheim, Norway, in the year 2001 and is primarily interested in 
computational morphology and syntax.





-----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-16-1351	

	



More information about the LINGUIST mailing list