16.841, Review: Phonology: van de Weijer et al. (2003)

LINGUIST List linguist at linguistlist.org
Sat Mar 19 20:25:58 UTC 2005


LINGUIST List: Vol-16-841. Sat Mar 19 2005. ISSN: 1068 - 4875.

Subject: 16.841, Review: Phonology: van de Weijer et al. (2003)

Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Wayne State U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
            Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>
 
Reviews (reviews at linguistlist.org) 
        Sheila Collberg, U of Arizona  
        Terry Langendoen, U of Arizona  

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org/

The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University, Wayne
State University, and donations from subscribers and publishers.

Editor for this issue: Naomi Ogasawara <naomi at linguistlist.org>
================================================================  

What follows is a review or discussion note contributed to our 
Book Discussion Forum. We expect discussions to be informal and 
interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially 
invited to join in. If you are interested in leading a book 
discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available 
for review." Then contact Sheila Collberg at collberg at linguistlist.org. 

===========================Directory==============================  

1)
Date: 17-Mar-2005
From: Carolina González < sauce at euskalnet.net >
Subject: The Phonological Spectrum, Vol. II: Suprasegmental Structure 

	
-------------------------Message 1 ---------------------------------- 
Date: Sat, 19 Mar 2005 15:23:35
From: Carolina González < sauce at euskalnet.net >
Subject: The Phonological Spectrum, Vol. II: Suprasegmental Structure 
 

EDITORS: van de Weijer, Jeroen; van Heuven, Vincent J.; van der Hulst, Harry
TITLE: The Phonological Spectrum
SUBTITLE: Volume II: Suprasegmental structure
SERIES: Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 234
PUBLISHER: John Benjamins
YEAR: 2003
Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/14/14-772.html


Carolina González, Department of Linguistics, University of California, 
Los Angeles

BRIEF SUMMARY

This is a collection of ten papers on suprasegmental phonology. The volume 
opens with a short, very general preface on the aims and contents of the 
two volumes of The Phonological Spectrum. After this, a note by the 
editors titled 'Syllables, feet and higher up', briefly discusses the 
organization and contents of the second volume. The remainder of the book 
is organized into three sections: (1) syllabic structure, (2) metrical 
structure, and (3) prosodic structure.

COMMENTS ON THE PAPERS

The first section of the book comprises four papers on syllable structure. 
In 'Phonetic Evidence for phonological structure in syllabification', 
Heather Goad and Kathleen Branner consider the representation of word 
final consonants in the earlier stages of language acquisition. The 
authors propose that word-final consonants (henceforth C#) are first 
syllabified as onsets regardless of whether they are onsets or codas in 
the adult stage. Departing from standard Government Phonology, the authors 
assume, following Itô (1986) and Piggott (1991, 1999), that C# can be 
syllabified as codas or as onsets of a syllable with an empty nucleus. 
This choice depends on various properties of the language, including 
segmental profiling (if C# are restricted in respect to word internal 
codas, they are onsets; otherwise, they are codas), rhyme shape (languages 
with word final codas allow CVC structures only; languages where C# are 
onsets allow CVXC structures), and release properties (C# with onset- like 
release properties will be onsets; final codas will usually be 
neutralized).

According to Goad and Branner, early acquisition of C# in English proceeds 
in two stages. In the first stage the syllable structure is CV; epenthesis 
after C# and word truncation are common. In the second stage C# are 
pronounced, but they are onsets, since they emerge before word-internal 
codas and show onset-like release properties (aspiration, nasal release, 
and lengthening; the preceding vowel may also be lengthened or have a post-
vocalic pause).

Goad and Branner propose that in early stages of acquisition C# syllabify 
as onsets regardless of the constraints of the target grammar. But this 
conclusion is based only on English, where the syllabification of C# in 
the adult stage is ambiguous; minimal words have a CVX shape, which 
suggest that C# have weight and are codas. On the other hand, CVXC are 
allowed at the right edge, which argues for a syllabification of C# as the 
onset of an empty headed syllable. This is acknowledged by the authors 
(footnote 6, p. 26) but the implications for their analysis are not 
discussed. Additionally, Goad and Branner propose that C# forms a rhyme 
unit with the following empty nucleus, this structure being most 
consistent with the release and timing properties found in this position. 
It is unclear that this structure is unmarked enough to be posited at this 
stage. Finally, the reference title for Hildegard's data, Leopold (1939), 
mentions that Hildegard is a bilingual child; this is not discussed in the 
paper either.

'The phonology-phonetics interface and Syllabic Theory', by Shosuke 
Haraguchi expands and revises Haraguchi 1998 Set Theory of the Syllable 
(STS). This theory assumes that syllables consist underlyingly of 
unordered sets of features, with ordering determined by principles of 
linearization and phonetic implementation. Syllable structure is 
parametrized, with the unmarked syllable composed of the 'core' (onset and 
nucleus) and the 'coda', as in Japanese, and the marked case formed 
of 'onset' and 'rhyme' (nucleus and coda),as in English. The unmarked 
syllable structure explains the universal preference for CV syllables. 
Various universal constraints determine the principles of linearization, 
including that the onset precedes the nucleus, and the nucleus precedes 
the coda.

Haraguchi considers a number of properties of linearization and the 
phonology-phonetics interface in Tokyo Japanese and English. In Tokyo 
Japanese the syllable template is (C)V (V)(X), where X is either a moraic 
nasal or the first half of a geminate. Restrictions in what constitutes a 
possible syllable in this language make underlying unordering possible. 
Additionally, the morphological structure of words is responsible for 
determining the relative ordering of verbal roots and suffixes, as 
exemplified by the alternations found in inflectional verbal forms in 
Japanese.

Haraguchi defends that STS captures various facts in English without the 
need for additional machinery. For example, since /h/ and /ng/ are 
restricted to onset and coda positions respectively, any word with these 
segments will consist on a sequence of unordered segments with only one 
possible interpretation. However, linearization has to apply at some point 
in the phonology to allow for cases where segments need to be adjacent, as 
in voicing assimilation in the plural or in the past tense. Haraguchi 
concludes that STS accounts for a number of previously unnoticed or 
unexplained facts. A number of issues remain to be investigated, as 
pointed out in p. 55; among them, to make the system of linearization more 
explicit and to examine languages with more complicated syllable 
structures.

'Hungarian as a strict CV language' by Krisztina Polgárdi, argues for an 
abstract representation of syllabic structure in Hungarian as CV, 
following Lowenstamm (1996). This is based on two pieces of evidence. 
First, there is an almost complete lack of phonotactic restrictions 
between word-internal consonant clusters; clusters where the second member 
is more sonorous than the first are very common. This is analyzed in 
standard Government Theory as two single onsets separated by an empty 
nucleus. Secondly, long vowels cannot precede consonant clusters. This is 
explained if an empty nucleus inhibits the preceding vowel from being 
long, since long vowels are represented as a vowel expanding to the next 
nucleus position. However, long /a/ and /e/ are exceptional in that they 
can precede clusters. Polgárdi shows that these vowels differ both 
qualitatively and quantitatively from their short counterparts; they also 
are unique in that they can undergo low vowel lengthening and alternate in 
shortening stems. Polgárdi analyzes long /a/ and /e/ as sequences of short 
vowels separated by an empty onset. This explains their occurrence before 
clusters. Polgárdi concludes that more surface-based accounts cannot 
explain important generalizations in Hungarian.

'Syllable structure at different levels in the speech production process: 
Evidence from aphasia', by Dirk-Bart den Ouden and Roelien Bastiaanse, 
reports an experimental study on syllable simplifications in aphasic 
patients. This study attempts to establish the extent to which syllable 
structure has a phonetic basis. Two groups of aphasics were tested: non-
fluent patients, which are commonly assumed to have a phonetic deficit, 
and fluent aphasics, which are supposed to have a more abstract 
phonological deficit. Their performance on a repetition task concerning 
words with different onset/coda combinations was contrasted; the results 
show that both groups of aphasics behave similarly, producing more errors 
in codas than in onsets, and more in marginal syllabic elements. The 
findings suggest that both groups of aphasics have similar syllable 
representations, and that syllable structure plays a role at both the 
(cognitive) phonetic level and the phonological level. 

The section on metrical structure opens with 'Quantity- sensitivity of 
syllabic trochees revisited: The case of dialectal gemination in Finnish', 
by Heli Harrikari. In various eastern and northern Finnish dialects, 
gemination targets the medial consonant of a (CV.CVV) sequence, giving 
rise to the structure (CVC.CVV). Harrikari proposes that this is foot-
sensitive gemination that occurs to repair the worst type of trochee in 
Finnish (CV.CVV is an uneven trochee with a heavy syllable in non-head 
position) in order to create a more balanced trochee. Harrikari analyzes 
this phenomenon in Optimality Theory, with the crucial ranking of IDENT-
Nuc-syllable stress (a positional faithfulness constraint which penalizes 
changes in the moraic structure of the stressed vowel) over trochaic-
oriented constraints. This interaction explains other cases where 
gemination is preferred over vowel lengthening, even if is more marked. 
According to Harrikari, syllable weight is relevant in syllabic trochees 
in Finnish, which calls for the reevaluation of the general notion that 
syllabic trochees are quantity insensitive.

'Ternarity is Prosodic binarity' by Jay I. Rifkin, analyzes ternary 
structures as prosodic words, building on Itô and Mester (1992) and Hewitt 
(1992). Rifkin proposes that prosodic words are binary: they can be 
composed of two feet or a foot and an unparsed syllable. This is encoded 
as the constraint Bin (P).Rifkin exemplifies his proposal with the 
analysis of ternary structures in Cayuvava and Pirahã. In Cayuvava, a 
three-syllable iteration continues across the stress contour domain, and 
various segmental processes that interact with the prosody evidence the 
emergence of optimal prosodic words. Vowel deletion is blocked when it 
would result in a less optimal prosodic word. Additionally, semivowels j, 
w and vowels i, u are allophonic; j and w occur if there are already three 
vowels in the word, and i, u occur if there are only two vowels. This 
process reduces the number of unfooted syllables in the word. In Pirahã, 
word boundaries are left aside to create a better prosodic word, which 
provides evidence for the status of Bin (P). Rifkin suggest to extend this 
model to binary and quaternary systems (as Palestinian Arabic), and 
proposes that Bin (P) can substitute the Lapse constraint, and that feet 
can only be maximally binary. 

'The status of word stress in Indonesian', by Ellen van Zanten, Rob 
Goedemans and Jos Pacilly, reports a number of production and perception 
experiments designed to determine the location of word stress in 
Indonesian. The production study was conducted on two Indonesian speakers, 
one with a Javanese substratum, and the other with a Toba Batak 
Substratum. These speakers produced four and five syllable words in 
sentence final focus and non-final non-focus position. The perception 
experiments also tested Indonesian speakers with different substrata (Toba 
Batak, Javanese, or Jakartan Indonesian/Malay). The results show that 
stress can be final or penultimate in Indonesian, suggesting that word 
stress is free or that it does not apply. Additionally, it was found that 
the substrate makes a difference in stress realization and perception 
(speakers with a Toba Batak substratum have strictly penultimate stress). 
The authors speculate that prominence in Javanese is phrase based, with 
one phrasal accent located near the end of the phrase.

The third section consists on three experimental papers on different 
aspects of the prosody of Dutch. 'Perceived prominence and the metrical-
prosodic structure', by Karijn Helsloot and Barbertje M. Streefker, 
reports the results of a perceptual experiment on word prominence in Dutch 
and proposes a model that incorporates these results to improve text to 
speech systems. Ten subjects listened to a number of sentences in Dutch 
and indicated which words were realized with emphasis. The results were 
mapped with theories of prosodic phonology, specifically with the 
relational-based metrical grid representation of Liberman and Prince 
(1977). Four levels of prosodic structure were found: (i) syllable, (ii) 
weakly stressed syllables(function words/non-head long context words, head 
syllables of verbs), (iii) lexically stressed head syllables of nouns, and 
(iv) head syllables of adjectives, adverbs, negative particles and 
deictically used demonstrative pronouns. These four levels were captured 
through a series of prosodic input constraints. The experiment also 
provided evidence for an increase of prominence in the first prominent 
word in the sentence, deletion of the last highest prominence in the 
sentence, and avoidance of clashes and lapses. These facts were captured 
through prosodic output constraints, which are higher ranked than prosodic 
input constraints; among these, avoidance of lapses and clashes are ranked 
highest.

'Phonetic variation or phonological difference? The case of the early 
versus the late accent-lending fall in Dutch', by Johanneke Caspers, 
reports a perceptual experiment that tested whether different melodic 
differences in Dutch are relevant phonologically. The study involved 
pairwise and rating tasks aiming to see if subjects distinguish different 
meanings and attitudes in the use of two types of falling pitch accents: 
early vs. late accent lending falls. The results support the hypothesis 
that a difference in timing relates to a difference in meaning; the early 
accent is more prone to project information , while late accent stresses 
new information. Additionally, there is a difference in perceived 
attitude, with the early accent-lending fall perceived as 'less 
acceptable, more detached, more final and more irritated' than the late 
accent-lending fall (p. 217). These results suggest that both pitch 
accents are separate phonological categories. 

'On the categorical nature of intonational contrasts- an experiment on 
boundary tones in Dutch', by Bert Remijsen and Vincent J. van Heuven, 
closes the volume. This paper reports an experiment that tested whether 
the low and the high boundary tone, which in Dutch signal the end of a 
statement and a question marker respectively, are distinguished 
continuously or categorically. The results from an identification and 
discrimination task show that there is a categorical distinction between 
the two boundary tones. This provides evidence for the phonological status 
of these tones and constitutes a clear example of categorical perception 
of intonational contrasts.

GENERAL COMMENTS AND SUMMARY

The papers represent diverse theoretical and experimental approaches to 
suprasegmental structure. The range of languages investigated is quite 
varied, and includes English, Japanese, Hungarian, Finnish, Indonesian, 
Cayuvava, Pirahã and Dutch. There is a good balance between experimental 
and theoretical contributions. However, the choice of experimental papers 
could have been more diverse; four out of the five contributions focus on 
the Dutch language.

REFERENCES

Haraguchi, Shosuke. 1998. A Theory of the Syllable. Paper presented at 
LP'98, Columbus, Ohio, September 15, 1998.

Hewitt, M. 1992. Vertical maximization and metrical theory. Ms., Brandeis 
University.

Itô, Junko. 1986. Syllable theory in prosodic phonology. Ph.D. 
Dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Published 1988 by New 
York: Garland.

Itô, Junko and Armin Mester. 1992. Weak layering and word binarity. Ms., 
UC Santa Cruz.

Leopold, W. F. 1939. Speech development of a bilingual child; a linguist's 
record. New York: AMS Press.

Liberman, Mark and Alan Prince. 1977. On stress and linguistic rhythm. 
Linguistic Inquiry 8. 249-336.

Lowenstamm, J. 1996. CV as the only syllable type. In J. Durand & B. Laks 
(eds.), Current Trends in Phonology: Models and Methods . CNRS, ESRI, 
Paris X. 419-441.

Piggott, G. L. 1991. Apocope and the licensing of empty-headed syllables. 
The Linguistic Review 8, 287-318.

Piggott, G. L. 1999. At the right edge of words. The Linguistic Review 16, 
143-185. 

ABOUT THE REVIEWER

Carolina González is currently a visiting scholar in the Department of 
Linguistics at UCLA. Her interests include metrically-conditioned 
phenomena and experimental research on the factors that influence the 
pronunciation of consonants.





-----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-16-841	

	



More information about the LINGUIST mailing list