16.2747, Review: Syntax/European Langs: Kiss & Riemsdijk (2004)

LINGUIST List linguist at linguistlist.org
Sat Sep 24 20:10:16 UTC 2005


LINGUIST List: Vol-16-2747. Sat Sep 24 2005. ISSN: 1068 - 4875.

Subject: 16.2747, Review: Syntax/European Langs: Kiss & Riemsdijk (2004)

Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Wayne State U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
            Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>
 
Reviews (reviews at linguistlist.org) 
        Sheila Dooley, U of Arizona  
        Terry Langendoen, U of Arizona  

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org/

The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University, Wayne
State University, and donations from subscribers and publishers.

Editor for this issue: Lindsay Butler <lindsay at linguistlist.org>
================================================================  

What follows is a review or discussion note contributed to our 
Book Discussion Forum. We expect discussions to be informal and 
interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially 
invited to join in. If you are interested in leading a book 
discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available 
for review." Then contact Sheila Dooley at dooley at linguistlist.org. 

===========================Directory==============================  

1)
Date: 20-Sep-2005
From: Michael Wagner < chael at mit.edu >
Subject: Verb Clusters: A study of Hungarian, German and Dutch 

	
-------------------------Message 1 ---------------------------------- 
Date: Sat, 24 Sep 2005 15:45:26
From: Michael Wagner < chael at mit.edu >
Subject: Verb Clusters: A study of Hungarian, German and Dutch 
 

EDITORS: Kiss, Katalin É.; Riemsdijk, Henk van 
TITLE: Verb Clusters
SUBTITLE: A study of Hungarian, German and Dutch.
SERIES: Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 69
PUBLISHER: John Benjamins
YEAR: 2004
Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/15/15-1851.html 

INTRODUCTION

This volume comprises 14 articles on verb clustering and related 
phenomena in Hungarian and West Germanic and an introduction to 
the topic by the editors. It contributes to the substantial body of work 
on verb clustering within generative grammar in the 30 years since 
Evers (1975) suggested that verbal complexes are constituents 
formed by adjoining embedded verbs to matrix verbs. The articles 
report on the results of a project funded in collaboration by the 
Netherlands and Hungary.

Verb clusters consist of sequences of predicates that form a 
selectional chain: The highest predicate or auxiliary (Pred-1) takes an 
infinitival or participial complement (Pred-2) which in turn may take a 
predicate as its complement (Pred-3) and so on. Clusters may also 
contain a particle, which are in general the complement (or within the 
complement) of the 'deepest' predicate in the selectional chain.

In a language like English, predicates are ordered left-to-right 
according to the selectional sequence:

(1) English
(that) we should-1 want-2 to have-3 to let-4 him solve-5 this problem

Verb-Clustering languages such as Dutch, German, and Hungarian 
differ from English in that the predicates seemingly form a constituent. 
While in English, arguments intersperse with the predicates (e.g. 'him' 
in the example above), in Dutch and German, all arguments precede 
the entire predicate complex (Kiss and van Riemsdijk, Introduction). I 
coindex the arguments with the predicates they are selected by:

(2) Dutch (1-2-3-4-5 'English Order' as in ex. (1))
dat wij hem-4 dit probleem zouden-1 willen-2 moeten-3 laten-4 
oplossen-5.
that we him   the problem  should   want     have.to  let     solve

In this example, the predicates within the cluster are ordered with 
respect to each other just like in English: 1-2-3-4-5. Also, the 
arguments are ordered with respect to each other like in English. But 
in Dutch, both nominal arguments precede the entire predicate 
complex, while in English the nominal arguments directly follow the 
selecting predicate.

The ordering in Dutch results in cross-serial dependencies (cf. 
Bresnan et al 1982). This type of structure that entered the spotlight 
of computational linguistics in the 80s since it cannot (if unbounded) 
be generated by context-free grammars (see, e.g., discussion in 
Shieber 1985).

German, also a clustering language, differs from Dutch in showing the 
inverse linear order among the predicates. This order (5-4-3-2-1) is 
called 'Roll-up Order' by many contributions in the volume:

(3) German (5-4-3-2-1 'Roll-Up Order')
dass wir ihn dieses Problem lösen-5 lassen-4 müssen-3 wollen-2 
können-1
that we  him this   problem solve   let      have.to  want     can

The third type of linear order apart from the 'English Order' and 
the 'Roll-up Order' that plays a major role in the volume is the 'Particle 
Climbing order' illustrated in (4), where the deepest constituent 
precedes all other predicates, which in turn are ordered in the 'English 
Order':

(4) Dutch (3-1-2 'Particle Climbing' Order)
dat  zij hem op wilde  bellen
that she him up wanted call

The three word order options observed in Dutch and German 
clusters -- 'English Order', 'Roll-up Order', and 'Particle Climbing' -- 
are all attested in Hungarian. Part of the goal of the volume is to 
explore parallels and differences between clusters in West Germanic 
and Hungarian.

In Hungarian, only non-finite verbs can roll up, so that the roll-up in 
(5b) is not complete and the finite verb is ordered first (Szendroi & 
Tóth, this volume):

(5) a. Hungarian 1-2-3-4 ('English Order')
Kedden  fog-1 tudni-2 jarni-3 dezeni-4.
Tuesday will  can     go      train
'He will be able to go training on Tuesdays.'

(5) b. Hungarian 1-4-3-2 ('Partial Roll-Up')
Kedden  fog-1 dezeni-4 jarni-3 tudni-2.
Tuesday will  train    go      can

(5) c. Hungarian 3-1-2 ('Particle-Climbing'; from Kiss and van 
Riemsdijk)
Janos fel-3 szeretne-1 hivni-2 Marit
Janos up    would.like call    Marit

The introduction by Katalin É. Kiss and Henk van Riemsdijk 
summarizes the main properties of clusters (e.g. adjacency effects, 
clause union effects, possible/impossible linear permutations, 
morphological properties) and lays out some of the main strands of 
ideas in the theoretical literature on the topic, many of which will come 
up in the discussion of the individual contributions below.

I will henceforth use numbers to refer to the predicates in a cluster, 
labeling the highest predicate in the selectional chain as 1, the next 
one down 2 and so on. 

DISCUSSION

I structure the discussion section of this review according to the 
subheadings used in the volume: DATA, THEORIES, PROSODY, 
ASPECT, VO/OV and MORPHOLOGY. For reasons of space I only 
report on some salient points for each article, I cannot go into an 
equal amount of detail for each one. This selective attention is not 
intended as a comment on the value of the contributions, but simply 
due to the fact that the length and depth of the volume make a 
thorough review of each piece impossible in this format.

DATA

Susi Wurmbrand: West Germanic Clusters: The Empirical Domain
This article summarizes results from a study of variation in linear order 
in predicate clusters across a number of dialects of German (classified 
as 'German', 'Austrian', 'Vorarlberg', and 'Swiss'). In this study 
participants were asked (i) for their favored order of certain predicate 
clusters with 3 or 4 members, and (ii) for their acceptability rating of a 
choice of orders. It is one the few existing empirical studies on 
variation in clustering (see also Hsiao (1999), Schmid and Vogel 
(2004), Seiler (2004)).

Several results of this study directly bear on generalizations taken as 
a given in some of the earlier literature. First, the results show that the 
IPP (infinitivus pro participio) cannot be causally linked to verb-
clustering and verb-reordering. This claim made was made in some 
earlier studies based on the suggestive fact that Frisian shows 
rigid 'roll-up' order (i.e. 3-2-1) and lacks IPP-effects.

Wurmbrand argues that the IPP neither depends on reordering of 
predicates (there are IPP-effects both in the order 3-2-1 and 1-2-3, at 
least one of which is the uninverted order in any theory), nor does 
reordering necessarily go along with IPP effects. Reordering is also 
possible in some auxiliary-modal constructions which involve infinitives 
in the first place (see also Kathol 1998 for this point).

Second, the issue of which predicate orders are possible/impossible is 
taken up. It seems that each permutation of three predicates is 
possible at least in some dialects. Cases of 2-1-3, however, are rare, 
and Wurmbrand proposes that existing cases of 2-1-3 involve 
extraposition of 3. Extraposition constructions certainly do create 2-1-
3 orders, as is illustrated in the following example:

(6) German 2-1-3 (Extraposition)
dass sie versucht-2 hat-1 zu schweigen-3
that she tried      has   to be.silent 
'that she has tried to be silent'

Wurmbrand proposes to derive possible predicate orders using a post-
syntactic reordering operation, following Haegeman and Riemsdijk 
(1986). In particular, she proposes to invoke a linear order 'flip' (cf. 
Williams, the same volume, and Kathol 1998 for an HPSG proposal 
with similar properties) which inverts linear orders between sisters.

This operation is capable of generating the orders 3-2-1, 2-3-1, 1-2-3, 
and 1-3-2. But it cannot generate the orders 2-1-3 and 3-1-2, which 
hence must be derived by some other reordering process in narrow 
syntax -- according to Wurmbrand extraposition of 3 in the case of 2-1-
3 and phrasal leftward movement of 3 in the case of 3-2-1.

The motivation of this distinction between PF-reordering vs. narrow-
syntax reordering remains somewhat unclear. Wurmbrand (2004) 
takes up this issue and argues on independent grounds for the post-
syntactic nature of the 'Flipping' of predicates on the one hand, and 
the syntactic nature of the two additional reorderings that generate 3-
1-2 and 2-1-3 on the other.

Wurmbrand's proposal captures all possible word orders in the 
respective dialects (except the distribution of the 3-1-2 and 2-1-3 
orders) by lexically specifying which types of predicates (auxiliaries, 
modals) trigger a 'flip' with their complement, and whether or not 
the 'flip' is obligatory or optional for the particular class of embedding 
predicate, thus providing an elegant account of the dialectal variation.

The article only discusses the analysis of predicate clusters with 3 
elements. An interesting question that is left open for future inquiry is 
how the account deals with the data reported in the same article for 
predicate clusters with 4 elements. While the results of the survey of 
clusters with 4 elements are preliminary, one clear result seems to be 
that the order 1-4-2-3 (which cannot be derived by 'flip') is the 
preferred neutral word order for certain predicate sequences. This 
seems somewhat unexpected, since 1-4-2-3 is predicted to involve a 
syntactically motivated reordering by phrasal movement (focus 
according to Wurmbrand 2004, following the results of the survey in 
Schmid and Vogel (2004)), and should not be derivable as a result of 
PF-movement in the neutral case.

A related question that eventually needs to be addressed can be 
illustrated by Hungarian: only 3 of the 4 orderings derivable by 'flip' 
are attested in predicate clusters with 4 elements in 'roll-ups' where 
the highest predicate comes first: 1-2-3-4, 1-2-4-3, 1-4-3-2. The order 
1-3-4-2 -- i.e. the order where the 'roll-up' does not start at the lowest 
node -- is unattested. Why would 1-3-4-2 be special and be excluded 
as opposed to the other three? This issue arises not only in 
Hungarian, a similar pattern was reported for a German dialect in 
Bech (1955).

Kriszta Szendroi and Ildikó Tóth: Hungarian verbal clusters
Hungarian verb clusters occur in two types of sentences. The first 
type is called 'non-neutral', these are sentences which contain 
contrastive focus or sentential negation. They typically display a 
partial roll-up order (e.g. a cluster with order 1-4-3-2 or 1-2-4-3).

(7) Hungarian Partial Roll-up in Non-Neutral Sentences: 1-4-3-2 and 1-
2-4-3

The rolled-up predicates have to be adjacent and cannot be 
separated with adverbials or arguments. The finite predicate cannot 
roll up, and is thus always initial. If the highest predicate is nonfinite, a 
complete roll-up is possible. According to the judgments in the 
literature, the roll-up has to start at the bottom, but need not invert all 
predicates.

The second type of sentences are called 'neutral'. These typically 
include predicate clusters in the particle climbing order (e.g. a cluster 
with order 4-1-2-3 where 4 is the particle). Particle Climbing orders 
are e.g. the following ('Particle' is also called 'Verbal Modifier' 
or 'Preverb'; (4=Particle)):

(8) Hungarian Particle Climbing in Neutral Sentences: 4-1-2-3, 1-4-2-
3, and 1-2-4-3

The predicates following the particle are in the 'English order'. They 
can be interspersed with adverbials and quantifiers, but not with focus 
sensitive operators (e.g. 'only') or sentential negation.

This study, again based on a questionnaire, was designed to test the 
claims about possible word orders reported in the literature. It seems 
to be the first empirical study of word order possibilities in Hungarian 
verb clusters.

For 'non-neutral' sentences, the pattern reported in earlier literature 
was by and large confirmed:

(9) Judgments reported in the literature
1-2-3-4  (English Order)
1-2-4-3  (Partial Roll-up)
1-4-3-2  (Roll-Up (all predicates except finite one)
*1-3-2-4 (Partial Roll-Up in the middle)
*1-3-4-2 (Partial Roll-Up starting at higher node)
*1-4-2-3 (non-local Roll-Up)

The three orders claimed to be grammatical in the literature were 
rejected in less than 40% or the responses, while the orders claimed 
to be ungrammatical were rejected in more than 80% of the cases. 
The troubling result is the frequency with which participants rejected 
certain word orders that are generally assumed to be grammatical, at 
least for certain particular predicates. In particular, the 'English order' 
does not seem to be accepted by all speakers.

For 'neutral' sentences, orders involving full (e.g. 4-1-2-3 where 4 is 
the particle) and partial particle climbing (1-4-2-3) were tested. Partial 
climbing was more acceptable in cluster with 3 elements that in 
clusters with 4 elements. Other significant factors determining the 
acceptability of word orders were the predicate class and the 
transparency of the semantic relation between a predicate and its 
complement particle.

THEORIES

Jonathan Bobaljik: Clustering Theories
This article summarizes some of the main theoretical problems posed 
by predicate clusters. One interesting issue raised is why predicate 
clusters occur in Germanic OV but not in VO languages.

For example, cross-serial dependencies with respect to the 
distribution of arguments have not been reported (to my knowledge) 
for Germanic VO languages. In other words, while (10a) is attested in 
Dutch, (10b) has not been observed:

(10) 
a. Arg1 Arg2 Arg3 Pred1 Pred2 Pred3
b. *Pred1 Pred2 Pred3 Arg1 Arg2 Arg3

If this gap turns out to be systematic, it poses a challenge to what 
Bobaljik calls inheritance-based approaches, which allow the 
formation of complex predicates (e.g. Forward Partial Combination in 
Steedman (1985, 533), also Williams' 'Reassociate', this volume). The 
idea in those approaches is that predicates combine first to form a 
complex predicate and then subsequently take their arguments:

(11) Arg1 Arg2 [Pred1 + Pred2]

The question that Bobaljik raises is why, after forming the complex 
predicate, it has to take the arguments to its *left*. A language in 
which it takes both arguments to the *right* is not attested.

Bobaljik points out that theories that form clusters by reanalysis of a 
surface string (e.g. Haegeman and Riemsdijk 1985) might be able to 
account for the distribution. The reanalysis approach requires 
adjacency before forming the complex predicate. This is only given in 
OV order:

(12) Arg-1 Arg-2 Arg-3 Pred-3 Pred-2 Pred-1

After reanalysis of the string of predicates as one constituent, (post-
syntactic) rules of permutations derive different cluster word orders. 
Linear Adjacency, according to Bobaljik, might play a crucial role then 
in the derivation of predicate clusters, since it is a precondition on 
restructuring.

It is not clear however whether the generalization tying verb clustering 
to the OV-property is entirely correct -- at least if we also take adverbs 
into account, and not just arguments. One of the criteria for cluster 
formation is the linearization of adverbs relative to the predicates that 
they modify. Haider (2003) notes that adverbs, just like arguments, 
precede the entire predicate cluster and cannot be interspersed, while 
in English they are adjacent to the modified predicate:

(13) a. The new theory certainly may possibly have indeed been badly 
formulated (Quirk et al. 1986, 85)
(13) b. 
dass die Theorie wohl tatsächlich schlecht formuliert worden sein mag
that the theory possibly indeed   badly    formulated been   be   may

In English, the adverbs intersperse with the predicates, whereas in 
Cluster-languages, the adverbs are ordered in the same way with 
respect to each other, but they precede the predicate complex:

(14) a. English: Adv-1 Pred-1 Adv-2 Pred-2 Adv-3 Pred-3
(14) b. German:  Adv-1 Adv-2 Adv-3 Pred-3 Pred-2 Pred-1
(14) c. Dutch:   Adv-1 Adv-2 Adv-3 Pred-1 Pred-2 Pred-3

Once again, Dutch shows cross-serial dependencies.

Nilsen (2002, p. 72) illustrates adverb-related clustering effects in 
Norwegian, a VO-language. Adverbs (as opposed to arguments) 
sometimes precede a sequence of predicates, yielding cross-serial 
dependencies, similar to those observed in Dutch (Adv-1 modifies 
Pred-1 etc.):

(14) d. Norwegian: Adv-1 Adv-2 Adv-3 Pred-1 Pred-2 Pred-3

It is unclear then how the reanalysis theory would derive clustering 
effects with respect to adverbials in a VO language such as 
Norwegian. In addition, Bobaljik's puzzle remains of why clustering of 
predicates with respect to arguments seems only to be attested in OV 
languages.

Michael Brody: Roll-Up Structures and Morphological Words
Brody's article illustrates how 'mirror theory' (Brody 2000) captures 
some of the differences between the roll-up construction on the one 
hand and the particle-climbing structure on the other.

The only way to derive a 'Roll-up' order (e.g. 1-4-3-2) in mirror theory 
is for the 'rolled-up' predicates to form a single morphological word. 
Within words, syntactic complements are specifiers, resulting in the 
inverted linearization. This is due to the assumption that specifiers (as 
opposed to complements) precede their sister.

The particle climbing order (e.g. 4-1-2-3) involves a long-distance 
dependency. Therefore it cannot be a single morphological word in 
Brody's theory and must involve a phrasal chain.

The fact that the predicate sequence in the particle climbing order can 
be interrupted by adverbs, but cannot in the roll-up structure (cf. also 
Koopman and Szabolci 1998) jibes well with mirror theory, since only 
the latter constitutes a morphological word, as can be seen in its linear 
ordering ('roll-up'). The article gives further evidence that particle-
climbing involves a phrasal chain, but roll-up does not.

Brody only discusses Hungarian data. Dutch predicate clusters show 
similar adjacency effects as Hungarian roll-up clusters, and are 
also 'word-like'. However, they have the 'English' word order. It is not 
clear then whether mirror theory might not be too restrictive and 
specifically designed to account for the Hungarian data.

Edwin Williams: The structure of clusters
Williams presents the set of structures CAT, from Williams (2002), 
which is argued to comprise all permutations of functional elements 
that are possible in natural language -- at least all those permutations 
that do not involve phrasal movement. CAT takes the Cinquean 
hierarchy of functional elements (F-seq) as a given.

The set of permutations that CAT allows can be described in the 
following way. Assuming that the basic order is Pred1 > Pred2 > 
Pred3 ..., the set of permutations can be derived by the 
operations 'flip' and 'reassociate'.

'Flip' swaps the linear order of two sisters, but keeps the functor-
argument relation constant, thus deriving 'left-branching' structures 
such as:
A > B ----------> B < A

'Reassociate' can re-bracket predicate sequences:
A > [ B > C] ---------> [ A > B] > C

Reassociate can feed flip, deriving [ B < A ] > C from A > [ B > C ]. But 
flip blocks any further application of Reassociate (i.e. Predicates 
combining by '<' cannot reassociate). Combinations of Flip and 
Reassociate can generate long-distance dependencies.

CAT is similar to combinatory categorial grammar (e.g. Steedman 
1985 for an application to predicate clusters) in that actual orderings 
that a particular language chooses are not derived by movement, but 
are due to lexical linear order information. The lexical entry specifies 
whether a predicate takes its complement to left/right. But Williams 
assumes phrasal movement in addition to the permutations provided 
by CAT, thus departing from the CCG program.

CAT comprises too many structures -- language specific constraints 
further restrict the set of possible permutations. Williams illustrates 
that CAT allows Verb Projection Raising constructions (Haegeman 
and Riemsdijk 1986), and how it accounts for predicate orders in 
Hungarian.

Restrictions on the role up-construction in Hungarian predicate 
clusters are explained by making a number of assumptions: 
(i) roll-ups are single words (similar to Brody); 
(ii) words contain root-level elements, but not other words or phrases; 
(iii) all auxiliaries are ambiguous between root-level and word-level; 
(iv) root level lexical entries are specified to take their complement to 
the left, word level are specified to take their complement to the right; 
(v) finite verbs can only be word-level; 
(vi) complex words cannot head phrases. Once these assumptions 
have been granted the right linear orders follow.

But this is hardly surprising, since essentially every linear precedence 
relation is stipulated in a lexical entry. CAT is only relevant in that 
each of the output linear orders in Hungarian (there are three: 1-2-3-
4, 1-2-4-3, 1-4-3-2) have to lie within the combinatorial power of CAT. 
This is not a strong argument for CAT, however. With verb clusters of 
4 elements, CAT predicts that 22 out of 24 orders are possible 
(according to Williams). So if one randomly picks three word orders 
out of the 24, the probability that it falls outside of CAT is just 0.24. 
Also, once one assumes with Williams that the highest predicate 
cannot flip, CAT actually generates *all* 6 possible permutations of the 
lower three predicates.

It also seems that there is no deep reason in the analysis why words 
should be linearized in one way and phrases in another. A mirror 
image of Hungarian would seem to lie in the scope of CAT: If all roots 
take their complements to the right, words take their complement to 
the left, then only predicate clusters of the following form would be 
allowed, where roll-up has to start from the top:

(15)
2-3-4-5-1 
3-4-5-2-1 
4-5-3-2-1

These orders can be derived by flipping the higher predicates with 
their sisters and maintaining the basic order between the lower 
predicates. This Hungarian*, it seems to me, could be derived simply 
by minimally changing Williams account by specifying roots as taking 
their complement to the right and words as taking their complement to 
the left. The theory thus seems much more powerful than e.g. Brody's, 
in which the difference of linearization of words vs. phrases is 
universally fixed.

Williams goes on to show that CAT cannot explain why particle-
climbing is incompatible with any amount of roll-up and can only occur 
if all predicates to the right of the particle are linearized in the 'English' 
order (e.g. 4-1-2-3). He concludes with many other authors in the 
volume that particle climbing must involve phrasal movement. Phrasal 
movement, however, falls outside of the domain of CAT.

The article concludes with an incisive discussion of Koopman and 
Szabolcsi's (2000) phrasal movement account of predicate clustering. 
This is the only discussion of an approach to verb clustering involving 
only phrasal movement.

PROSODY

Kriszta Szendroi: A stress based approach to climbing
Szendroi proposes that particle climbing is driven by the prosodic 
deficiency of certain auxiliaries and other stress-avoiding predicates. 
The idea is that stress-avoiding predicates resist the assignment of 
main stress in the cluster, the particle climbs to the position where 
main stress is assigned.

This operation of particle climbing is assumed to involve phrasal 
movement. Phrasal movement is generally costly, so a derivation with 
particle-climbing would in general be ruled out. However, these more 
costly derivations win out if a stress-avoiding predicate can end up 
destressed if the movement applies but not otherwise. In this case, the 
less economical derivation wins out. The system thus operates within 
a global economy approach, using transderivational comparisons (cf. 
Chomsky 1995).

A phonological motivation for particle climbing is also assumed in 
many other contributions in the volume (Csirmaz, Kiss, Gabor, 
Ackema, Olsvay).

Anikó Csirmaz: Particles and phonologically defective predicates
This article presents an insight into the deeper parallels between 
Dutch and Hungarian Predicate Clusters. Csirmaz shows evidence 
that Hungarian distinguishes phrasal particles from particles that are 
heads, and draws a parallel to a similar observation about Dutch in 
(Koster 1994) (see also Kiss, same volume, p. 346 for discussion of 
this observation). Only head-particles can occur within a verb cluster; 
phrasal particles (e.g. particles with a modifier) have to climb to the 
initial position, in Csirmaz' analysis the specifier of a PredP 
projection. 'Phrasal' and 'Head' are defined in terms of bare phrase 
structure, such that a single element that does not further project has 
both X0 and XP status. In both languages, Particles and other verbal 
modifiers move to a specifier position in a projection PredP. Heads, in 
addition, have the alternative option of incorporating into the predicate.

ASPECT

Gabor Alberti: Climbing for Aspect
Alberti sees a different force as the trigger for particle climbing: 
particles are taken to be aspectualizers, and they move to check an 
aspectual feature in AspP (following Pinon 1995). The feature driven 
grammar of aspectuality interacts with independent factors, e.g. 
phonological constraints disfavoring certain kinds of complex 
phonological words.

The contributions by Csaba Olsvay, Kriszta Szendroi and Anikó 
Csirmaz all take auxiliaries (and certain other predicates) to be 
phonologically defective; Olsvay suggests that they are also 
aspectually defective. Part of the motivation of particle climbing is the 
stress-avoiding property of auxiliaries (and certain other predicates), 
just as in Szendroi and Csirmaz, but the generalization about what 
can act as a particle according to Olsvay is the following: the stressed 
element preceding the finite auxiliary must always be the closest 
constituent having an aspectual feature.

Furthermore, predicates that avoid stress are characterized as 
elements that cannot 'represent the main assertion'. According to  Kiss 
(this volume), the  'Main assertion' is defined as the 'leftmost element 
of the predicate phrase'. This restriction is held responsible for the 
fact that auxiliaries are stress-avoiding.

These correlations prompt the question why there would be such a 
close connection between aspect and prosody, which unfortunately 
none of the papers in the volume addresses.

VO/OV

Katalin É. Kiss: Parallel Strategies of verbal complex formation in 
Hungarian and West Germanic.
This contribution points out interesting parallels between clustering in 
West Germanic and Hungarian, e.g. the observation that focus 
sensitive operators and negation are generally ruled out in 
the 'particle climbing' order (e.g. Part-1-2-3). This is true even in those 
WG languages that generally allow to break up clusters by such 
elements in other orders (West Flemish and the Swiss German 
spoken in Zürich).

The paper argues that underlyingly, predicate clusters should be 
taken to have the 'English order'. One argument for this view is that In 
Hungarian, Particle-Climbing is possible from embedded clauses 
separated by a complementizer from the matrix clause. In those cases, 
Kiss argues, leftward movement of the particle has to be involved. By 
analogy, the same analysis can be given for particle climbing more 
generally. This is taken to argue for a base with the 'English word 
order', at least in Hungarian. (This argument is countered in Ackema 
(same volume) by the claim that in Hungarian, a complementizer can 
incorporate into a predicate.)

Kiss concludes that the predicate clusters in West Germanic and 
Hungarian can thus only be derived in a parallel way if the base for 
both is assumed to be VO and not OV.

Peter Ackema: Do preverbs climb?
Ackema discusses the claim that 'particle-climbing' (e.g. orders of the 
form 4-1-2-3) is derived by leftward movement assuming a VO-base 
and shows various problems with this kind of approach.

Leftward dislocation of predicates in other cases does not generally 
seem to obey the same restrictions. É.g. V2 movement can strand a 
particle, while the fronting of a participle within the cluster cannot:

(15)
a. John called his lawyer up. (Transliterated from Dutch)
b. *that John his lawyer called has up. (Transliterated from Dutch)

Ackema's point is that the two types of dislocation (Preverb/Particle 
Climbing and V2-movement) should not both be analyzed in the same 
way (i.e. leftward head-movement). Indeed, particle climbing violates 
the Head-Movement Constraint, in that it targets the lowest instead of 
the highest head.

Most analyses in the volume would agree with the position that 
(15a,b) involve different kinds of dislocation. But while the other 
approaches take Particle-Climbing to be phrasal movement, Ackema 
defends an alternative analysis, which assumes an OV-base, and 
derives apparent preverb-climbing as preverb-stranding. In other 
words, it is not the particle that moves leftward but it is everything else 
that moves rightward.

Ackema adds the stipulation that the preverb cannot move on its own. 
This stipulation is independently motivated by the fact that particles in 
Dutch are also excluded from scrambling. This restriction against the 
scrambling or particles is not explained in those proposals that treat 
particle climbing as phrasal movement (e.g. most of the other 
proposals in the volume).

This article is the most comprehensive comparative treatment in the 
volume, and very meticulous in trying to work out the parallels and 
differences between Hungarian and Dutch particle-climbing 
constructions.

MORPHOLOGY

Huba Bartos: Verbal complexes and morphosyntactic merger
This paper presents predicate sequences that are scopally 
ambiguous, but both readings are linearized in the same way. The tool 
used to deal with this apparent mismatch in morphological bracketing 
and syntactic scope is morpho-syntactic merger (based 
on 'morphological merger' in Halle and Marantz 1993). The 
assumption is that each step of merge in syntax can be followed by 
morphological operations. The particular linear orders are achieved by 
stipulating predicates to be underlying specified as [+ suffix], thus 
triggering a reordering in morphology.

Ildikó Tóth: Infinitival complements of modals in Hungarian and in 
German
The paper addresses clause-union effects, a property closely linked to 
verb clustering. Tóth observes that German modals can embed both 
personal and impersonal passives; Hungarian modals cannot embed 
impersonal constructions. Another difference relates to case marking. 
Hungarian subjects embedded under modals receive dative case, 
while in German they can receive nominative case if they are the 
highest argument in the sentence.

Interestingly, raising predicates in Hungarian and German pattern like 
German modals. Impersonal constructions can be embedded under 
raising verbs such as 'seem', where nominative case is assigned by 
the matrix predicate to the embedded subject. Tóth analyzes the 
difference between the modals in the two languages by positing that 
German but not Hungarian modals are raising predicates. German 
modals are thus restructuring predicates and involve a mono-clausal 
structure.

Marcel den Dikken: Agreement and 'Clause Union'
This last contribution also addresses clause-union effects. Den Dikken 
starts out by illustrating that 'Clause Union' is not unitary phenomenon 
but involves a scale of properties, and depending on the embedding 
verb some but not other clause-union effects occur:

Clause Union Effects:
1. Preverb-climbing to the matrix clause
2. Definiteness agreement with embedded object (definiteness 
agreement is evidenced by the choice of a different set of subject 
agreement markers in the presence of a 3rd person definite object).
3. Person agreement with embedded object (e.g. choice of special 
agreement markers in the case of first person subjects and second 
person objects)

den Dikken links these three effects to three different syntactic factors, 
which are independently argued for:

1. the location of AspP (preverbs move to the specifier of AspP)
2. the location of vP/AgrOP (if this projection is high, then there will be 
definiteness agreement)
3. the presence/absence of an IP boundary between the matrix and 
the embedded clause (which blocks person agreement )

The analysis is based on an intricate web of observations about 
agreement in Hungarian, and manages to relate them to other 
syntactic facts. The proposal leads to the novel proposal that 
Hungarian has object clitics.

CONCLUSION

The volume constitutes an important contribution to the understanding 
of verb clusters and related phenomena, and serves to familiarize the 
reader with the state of the art with respect to the empirical evidence 
and the main theoretical issues.

The main lessons to be learned is that the field is far from converging 
on a solution to the problem, and that the phenomena that factor into 
the explanation (possible/impossible linear order permutations, clause-
union effects) are more complex and necessitate finer grained 
analyses than previous studies were assuming.

Overall, the discussion of the theoretical issues would have been 
sharpened by a more direct confrontation of different proposals and 
different theoretical frameworks, rather than (as is mostly the case, 
one exception is Bobaljik's contribution) merely juxtaposing very 
different frameworks.

A notable gap in the coverage of theoretical approaches is the lack of 
a discussion of the treatment of verbal clusters in categorial grammar 
(e.g. Steedman 1985) and in HPSG (e.g. Kathol 2000, Mueller 2002), 
although individual articles make reference to results from both lines 
of research. Another issue that is not addressed is that one underlying 
assumption made in the volume -- namely that predicate clusters form 
constituents -- has been challenged in Kroch and Santorini (1991).

REFERENCES

Bech, Gunnar (1955/1957). Studien zum deutschen Verbum infinitum. 
Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.

Bresnan, Joan, Ronald Kaplan, Stanley Peters and Annie Zaenen 
(1982). Cross-serial dependencies in Dutch, Linguistic Inquiry 13: 613-
635.

Brody, Michael (2000). Mirror theory. Linguistic Inquiry 31:29-56.

Cinque, G. (1993). A null theory of phrase and compound stress. 
Linguistic Inquiry 24, 239-298.

Evers, Arnold (1975). The Transformational Cycle of Dutch and 
German, Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Utrecht.

Evers, Arnold (2003). Verb clusters and cluster creepers. In Verb 
Constructions in German and Dutch, ed. by Pieter Seuren and Gerard 
Kempen, 43-89. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing 
Company.

Haider, Hubert (2003). V-clustering and clause union: Causes and 
effects. In Verb Constructions in German and Dutch, ed. by Pieter 
Seuren and Gerard Kempen, 91-126. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John 
Benjamins Publishing Company.

Hsiao, Franny P. (2000). Relating syntactic variation in West 
Germanic to checking parameters. In Proceedings of ConSOLE 8, ed. 
by Christine Czinglar, Katharina Koehler, Erica Thrift, Erik Jan van der 
Torre, and Malte Zimmermann, 155-169. Leiden: SOLÉ.

Kathol, Andreas (1998). Linearization of verb clusters in West 
Germanic. In Proceedings of the 16th Western Conference on 
Linguistics (WECOL 16, 1996), ed. by Vida Samiian, 149-161. Fresno: 
California State University at Fresno.

Kathol, Andreas (2000). Linear Syntax. New York/Oxford: Oxford 
University Press.

Koopman, Hilda, and Anna Szabolcsi (2000). Verbal Complexes. 
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 

Koster, Jan. 1994. Predicate incorporation and the word order of 
Dutch. In Paths towards Universal Grammar: Studies in honor of 
Richard S. Kayne, ed. by Guglielmo Cinque, Jan Koster, Jean-Yves 
Pollock, Luigi Rizzi, and Raffaella Zanuttini, 255-277. Washington, 
D.C.: Georgetown University Press.

Kroch, Anthony S. and Beatrice Santorini (1991). The derived 
structure of the West Germanic verb raising construction. In Robert 
Freidin, ed., Principles and parameters in comparative grammar 
(Current studies in linguistics 20). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 269-
338.

Mueller, Stefan (2002). Complex Predicates: Verbal Complexes, 
resultative constructions, and particle verbs in German. Stanford: CSLI.

Nilsen, Oystein (2002). Eliminating Positions. Syntax and semantics of 
sentence modification. PhD dissertation, Utrecht University.

Quirk, Randolph, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech and Jan 
Svartvik (1986). A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. 
London: Longman. 

Schmid, Tanja and Ralf Vogel (2004). Dialectal variation in German 3-
verb clusters: A surface-oriented OT-account. Journal of Comparative 
Germanic Linguistics 7: 235-274. 
http://www.ling.uni-potsdam.de/~rvogel/schmid-vogel04.pdf

Seiler, Guido (2004). On three types of dialect variation, and their 
implications for linguistic theory. Evidence from verb clusters in Swiss 
German dialects. In: Dialectology meets Typology, ed. by Bernd 
Kortmann Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Draft dated May 15, 2003 
available at http://www.ds.unizh.ch/gseiler/downloads/seiler3types.pdf

Shieber, Stuart M. (1985). Evidence Against the Context-freeness of 
Natural Language, Linguistics and Philosophy 8: 333-343.

Steedman, Mark (1985). Dependency and coordination in Dutch and 
English. Language 61.3. 523--568.

Wurmbrand, Susi (2004). Syntactic vs. post-syntactic movement In: 
Proceedings of the 2003 Annual Meeting of the Canadian Linguistic 
Association (CLA), ed. by Sophie Burelle and Stanca Somesfalean, 
284-295. http://wurmbrand.uconn.edu/research/files/CLA-SW.pdf

Wurmbrand, Susi (2005). Online Bibliography on Verb Clusters and 
verb (projection) raising. 
http://wurmbrand.uconn.edu/research/Bibliographies/verbclusterbib.html

ABOUT THE REVIEWER

Michael Wagner is currently a post-doc in the Department of Brain 
and Cognitive Sciences at MIT. He has done work on segmental 
phonology, the syntax and semantics of focus, and the prosody of 
predication and modification. His dissertation looks at the relation 
between syntactic recursion and prosodic structure.





-----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-16-2747	

	



More information about the LINGUIST mailing list