17.798, Review: Typology/Syn tax/Semantics: W älchli (2005)

linguist at LINGUISTLIST.ORG linguist at LINGUISTLIST.ORG
Thu Mar 16 03:51:03 UTC 2006


LINGUIST List: Vol-17-798. Wed Mar 15 2006. ISSN: 1068 - 4875.

Subject: 17.798, Review: Typology/Syntax/Semantics: Wälchli (2005)

Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Wayne State U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
            Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>
 
Reviews (reviews at linguistlist.org) 
        Sheila Dooley, U of Arizona  
        Terry Langendoen, U of Arizona  

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org/

The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University, Wayne
State University, and donations from subscribers and publishers.

Editor for this issue: Lindsay Butler <lindsay at linguistlist.org>
================================================================  

What follows is a review or discussion note contributed to our 
Book Discussion Forum. We expect discussions to be informal and 
interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially 
invited to join in. If you are interested in leading a book 
discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available 
for review." Then contact Sheila Dooley at dooley at linguistlist.org. 

===========================Directory==============================  

1)
Date: 13-Mar-2006
From: Alexandre Arkhipov < sarkipo at mail.ru >
Subject: Co-Compounds and Natural Coordination 

	
-------------------------Message 1 ---------------------------------- 
Date: Wed, 15 Mar 2006 22:43:49
From: Alexandre Arkhipov < sarkipo at mail.ru >
Subject: Co-Compounds and Natural Coordination 
 

AUTHOR: Wälchli, Bernhard
TITLE: Co-Compounds and Natural Coordination
SERIES: Oxford Studies in Typology and Linguistic Theory
PUBLISHER: Oxford University Press
YEAR: 2005
Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/16/16-2414.html 

Alexandre Arkhipov, Moscow State University

INTRODUCTION

Co-compounds (< coordinating compounds) are also known as 
dvandva compounds, pair words (Russian 'parnye slova'), and 
copulative compounds (German 'Kopulativkomposita'). Mordvin t'et'at-
avat 'parents', literally 'father-mother', is an example of co-compound. 
These are the expressions of natural coordination which appear as 
word-like, rather than phrase-like, formally tight coordination.

Natural (vs. accidental) coordination conjoins inherently semantically 
close concepts belonging to the same taxonomic level, in a way that 
the meaning of the whole construction is a conceptual unit which is 
closely related to the meaning of the parts and belongs to a 
superordinate taxonomic level in respect to the parts. Examples of 
natural coordination are 'brother and sister', 'fork and spoon', 'eat and 
drink' as opposed to 'my uncle and his book' or 'read and swim'. 
Natural coordination, like some other semantic types of coordination, 
often tends to be expressed by formally tight (vs. loose) constructions. 
Generally, the tightness of a construction correlates with the number 
of morphemes and boundaries intervening between its parts (see 
Chapter 3). For example, a coordination of two nouns is looser if they 
are connected by an overt coordination marker and if each one bears 
its own articles, inflection markers and adpositions; it is tighter with the 
articles omitted, no overt coordinator and a single inflection marker 
attached to the whole. Tight coordination patterns, then, may expose 
different features, being more word-like (cf. Georgian ''dá-
dzma'' 'siblings', literally 'sister-brother', without any morpheme 
intervening and bearing a single stress and a single final inflection) or 
more phrase-like (cf. English ''brother and sister'', two phonological 
words separated by a coordinator). 

Co-compounds (=CCs) are treated as a cross-linguistic class type, i.e. 
as a type of language-specific classes which share a number of 
recurrent features. CCs usually take a more word-like pattern, but 
rarely can all CCs in a given language be undoubtedly classified as 
words. Also, the formal properties of CCs vary across languages 
along with the properties of coordination patterns, hence the problem 
of definition (see Chapter 4).

The book investigates the formal, semantic and use-specific 
properties of CCs both in particular languages and cross-linguistically.

STRUCTURE

Chapter 1, 'Introduction', is an overview of the problems to be 
discussed and of methods to be used in the rest of the book. In 
Chapters 2 and 3 co-compounds are treated along with their phrase-
like counterpart. Chapters 4 to 7 consider co-compounds on their 
own. The book is accomplished with Conclusions (Chapter 8), 
Appendices showing genetic affiliation and location of the languages 
cited, Bibliography and Indices.

SYNOPSIS

Chapter 2, 'The marking patterns of natural coordination', makes a 
tour of the marking strategies used to code natural coordination. Two 
main options are relational and non-relational marking. Marking is 
relational if it is the relation between the coordinated elements 
(coordinands) which is coded by an overt marker (e.g. English ''and''). 
Natural coordination rarely uses overt markers of its own, but can also 
be distinguished by the absence or reduction of a 'normal' 
coordination marker. The absence of marker has, though, limited 
distinctive power, as in many languages coordination may be normally 
zero-marked.

Coordination has a non-relational marking if the markers used in the 
construction encode some properties of the coordinands which do not 
concern the relation of coordination itself; cf. the marking of 
definiteness, number, possession etc. on coordinand(s). This non-
relational marking may be distinctive if the patterns are different for 
different types of coordination. E.g. possession may be marked on 
both coordinands in ordinary coordination, but only once in natural 
coordination, as in Eastern Armenian; this is what Bernhard Wälchli 
(hereafter BW) calls 'distinctive non-relational single marking'. There 
are two more options, viz. double and zero marking. Non-relational 
double marking may involve double dual, plural, comitative, 
proprietive, additive focus or possessive marking. Systematic 
distinctive zero marking appears to be confined to the definiteness 
category, occasionally involving also number and adpositions. All 
three subtypes receive explanation in terms of concurrence between 
two iconic motivations: that of minimal distance and that of symmetry. 
The issue of syntactic (a)symmetry of single marking constructions is 
also discussed; it is argued that group inflection phenomena support 
the claim of non-isomorphism between phonology and syntax rather 
than the syntactic asymmetry of the construction.


Chapter 3, 'Tight coordination', explores the formal properties leading 
to the tight-loose contrast, and also the semantic nature of 
coordination that can be realized as tight coordination. The first formal 
parameter is the length of coordinands: the shorter the coordinands, 
the tighter is the construction. The second parameter of tightness 
pertains to the patterns of coordination: the less material (words, 
affixes, coordinators, intonation breaks etc.) is separating the two 
slots, the tighter is the pattern. One important observation is that 
typically a language does not have two strictly distinct patterns, a 
loose one and a tight one, but rather a whole range of patterns which 
occupy different places on the loose-to-tight continuum; Turkish 
nominal coordination is taken as an example of such a range of 
patterns. However, it is not always possible to decide unequivocally 
which one of two patterns is tighter or looser. Moreover, the degree of 
tightness of a construction is intertwined with the degree of 
grammaticalization of particular affixes and adpositions; if, say, a 
preposition is repeated with each coordinand in one case but is used 
only once in another, it is not always obvious whether the difference 
lies in the tightness of two constructions or in the degree of 
grammaticalization of the preposition.

The larger part of the chapter is devoted to various semantic types of 
coordination which can be expressed by tight constructions. Most of 
these types (group coordination, intersective coordination, 
enumeration, and pseudo-coordination) are shown to be independent 
of the natural coordination. E.g. group coordination may conjoin a 
natural pair, but this is not obligatory; at the same time, elements that 
naturally belong together may participate in a situation separately, as 
in distributive contexts (''Brother and sister went to different schools''). 
Some other kinds of coordination that turn out to be relevant for 
natural coordination are overlapping, non-exhaustive listing, and 
disjunctive coordination. The low degree of contrast between the 
coordinands, characteristic of natural coordination, also correlates 
with tighter constructions.

In Chapter 4, 'Co-compounds as a lexical class type', it is argued that 
co-compounds should be defined as a more basic concept than 
compounds; they are considered as a functional-formal class type. 
Discussing various problems connected to the definitions of word and 
compound (prosodic vs. grammatical words; non-compositionality 
or 'unitary' semantics of compounds; special compounding forms of 
stems; fixed order, etc.), the author decides not to dwell upon the 
notion of word or compound. The approach chosen here is to prove 
CCs to be a valid lexical class (vs. grammatical classes such as plural 
or future); examples of other lexical classes include middle as 
described by Kemmer (1993), diminutives, light verb with specifier 
constructions ('instruction + give > teach', 'work + do > to work'), or 
noun incorporation as described by Mithun (1984). Lexical classes 
typically have 'a clearly discernible semantic core'; they are highly 
idiosyncratic as to participation of individual elements, with lexeme-
internal vacillation; they also do interact with other lexical classes.

An important feature of a well-established lexical class is its 
productivity in two aspects: it should be productive in forming 
occasional highly context-oriented units, which, on the other hand, 
should be able to lexicalise and penetrate the permanent part of the 
lexicon. It is also essential that co-compounds as a lexical class ''have 
a strong tendency to be formally non-distinct from other linguistic 
phenomena'', such as sub-compounds, serial verbs, and phrase 
coordination. Even if the class of co-compounds in a given language is 
usually formally distinct from coordination, almost always one can find 
examples which cannot be unambiguously attributed to one of those.

In Chapter 5, 'A semantic classification of co-compounds', some ten 
semantic types of CCs are discussed. The classification is based on 
the semantic relation between the parts of CC and the whole 
compound. One and the same CC may be attributed to different types, 
depending on its actual meaning in a given context. Not all semantic 
types can be strictly distinguished. There are five basic types: 
(i) Additive CCs denote the sum of its parts (Georgian ''xel-p'exi'' 'hand-
foot').
(ii) Generalizing CCs are a sort of universal quantifiers, they denote 
general notions such as 'all', 'always' and 'everywhere', depending on 
the type of object referred to by their parts (Tagalog ''araw-gabi'' 'day 
and night'). The parts often have opposite meanings (day and night, 
big and small, here and there). 
(iii) Collective CCs represent collective types of which their parts 
denote typical representatives, but do not exhaustively list the whole 
(Chuvash ''erex-sara'' 'vodka/wine-beer > alcoholic beverages'). 
(iv) In alternative CCs the whole is the disjunction of the parts ('two-
three > two or three'). They occur even in weakly co-compounding, 
e.g. West European, languages. They are semantically close to the 
(v) approximate CCs ('two-three > several').

Several other types (synonymic, ornamental, imitative, figurative, and 
scalar CCs) are considered non-basic. The basic types conform 
strictly to the prototype of natural coordination, while each of non-
basic types deviates from the features of natural coordination (within 
part-part relations, part-whole relations, or the meaning of the whole). 
(vi) In synonymic CCs, the meaning of the whole is almost identical to 
the meanings of both parts (Khalkha Mongolian ''üzel bodol'' 'view-
thought > opinion'), thus is not taxonomically superordinate. 
(vii) In ornamental CCs, one of the parts does not contribute to the 
meaning of the whole, thus the two parts are not on the same 
taxonomic level (Mordvin ''vir'-ukstor'' 'forest-maple > forest'). 
(viii) Imitative CCs have a meaningless part phonologically similar to 
the meaningful part (Turkish ''çoluk çocuk'' '[IMITATIVE] child > wife 
and family'), so that there is no coordination strictly speaking. 
(ix) The meaning of figurative CCs belongs to another semantic 
domain than the meanings of the parts (Mandarin Chinese ''mao2 
dun4'' 'spear shield > contradictory, inconsistent'). 
(x) In scalar CCs there is maximal contrast between the parts, the 
whole denoting an abstract scale between these two extremes 
(Tibetan ''srab-mthug'' 'thin-thick > density'). 
The synchronic relations between different semantic types of CCs do 
not necessarily hold in diachrony: e.g. ornamental CCs do not derive 
from semantically close synonymic CCs. Several other types of 
compounds share similar properties with CCs, like appositional 
compounds (French ''wagon-restaurant''), intermediate compounds 
(English ''southwest''), echo words (Turkish ''doktor moktor'' 'doctor 
and/or the like') etc.; the closest to the CCs are ideophone 
compounds like English ''ding-dong''.

The last section of the chapter discusses the various semantic factors 
that favour the use of semantically redundant CCs, e.g. synonymic 
CCs: emphasis, generalization, contrast, non-referentiality under the 
scope of negation, and pictorality. These contextual factors may 
neutralize the differences between types of CCs and also between 
CCs and related types of compounds.

Chapter 6, 'Areal distribution in the languages of Eurasia', is devoted 
to the comparative frequency measurements of co-compounds in texts 
in various languages, principally those of Eurasia. The main 
observation of the chapter is that, with few exceptions, the ''co-
compounding'' decreases steadily from continental South-East Asia 
(Mandarin, Vietnamese, White Hmong) to the west (West European 
languages) as well as to the north. The main exceptions to this areal 
vector are Basque in the West which has a moderate level of co-
compounding (higher than its neighbours), and Modern Tamil and 
Modern Uyghur which have too few CCs.

The calculations have been made using two kinds of parallel texts: 
Universal Declaration of Human Rights or excerpts therefrom 
(=UDHR), and Gospel according to Mark (=Mark). Seven levels of co-
compounding are distinguished. It is shown that geographic location is 
sometimes more significant here than the genetic affiliation; e.g., 
genetically closely related Turkic languages do not behave so 
uniformly as the several distinct linguistic families in the Caucasus 
region.

Translations of Mark are also used to count the relative frequency of 
different semantic types of co-compounds. It is argued that the 
proportions of CC types in a given text depend more on the overall 
frequency of CCs than on other factors. There are some correlations; 
notably, the proportion of non-basic (synonymic + ornamental + 
imitative) CCs increases along with the overall frequency of CCs, i.e. 
in highly compounding languages, while the proportion of generalizing 
CCs decreases at the same time (their absolute number remaining 
roughly the same). These tendencies prove then to work in language-
internal variation (case study of Erzya Mordvin). Here two fiction texts 
and a traditional epos are compared, revealing great variation 
between the three in frequencies of CCs, with the proportions of their 
semantic types sticking to the predictions.

Finally, non-Eurasian languages are discussed in an appendix to 
Chapter 6. Beyond Eurasia, CCs are found in New Guinea and more 
scarcely, in the Americas (Quechua, Chinantec and Mixe, Tzotzil). 
They seem to be very rare in Africa and Australia, as well as in Creole 
languages.

In Chapter 7, 'Some considerations about the diachronic evolution of 
co-compounds', the two main perspectives on the evolution of CCs 
are discussed: their evolution as formal patterns vs. their evolution as 
lexical classes. The role of textual markedness in the destiny of CCs 
and their affinity to the folk poetry are also addressed. It is argued that 
normally formal patterns for CCs emerge from tight coordination 
patterns, although spontaneous evolution is not excluded (viz. for 
mere juxtaposition patterns). The class of CCs may also arise as an 
extension of tight coordination into word-domains, without introducing 
a new formal pattern. 

The formation of a class of CCs is different from the evolution of 
specific formal patterns which do not necessarily form classes. It is 
argued that lexical classes of CCs evolve gradually, so that every 
highly co-compounding language should have passed the stages of 
low and moderate level of co-compounding. What prevents CCs from 
being used in non-co-compounding languages is their high degree of 
textual markedness. The more CCs exist already in a language (or in 
a particular register or style), the easier it is for a new CC to lexicalise; 
so the development of a class of CCs is self-accelerating process. The 
same idea accounts for the fact that non-basic CCs begin their 
expansion only after a certain level of co-compounding is reached, as 
they are still more textually marked than the basic CCs.

EVALUATION

On the whole, the book reviewed here is well-structured and presents 
a fresh view upon a phenomenon important to many languages of the 
world, containing a considerable amount of language data. Co-
compounds turn out to be a cluster of formal and functional features, 
and consequently, the book should be studied carefully by linguists 
interested in a number of subjects such as compounding, 
coordination, comitative, associative plural, and lexical semantics 
within domains specific to co-compounding. A number of basic 
linguistic notions are reconsidered, such as word and compound, 
markedness and lexicalisation. One of the main points of the book is 
to show the importance of continuous variables in typology, which 
allow one to account for language-internal as well as cross-linguistic 
variation.

A good deal of the study is centred around Erzya Mordvin in which the 
author is proficient, but in whole the data of various languages are 
sufficiently balanced for this type of study as far as it does not pretend 
to use a representative sample of the world's language families.

The semantic classification of CCs is probably the most subtle issue in 
the book, and the author himself acknowledges repeatedly that no 
sharp border can be settled between some classes, as well as 
between CCs and some related phenomena. Some questions still 
remain controversial. Thus we find echo-words like Turkic m- and 
Yiddish shm-formations (cf. Turkish ''doktor moktor'' 'a doctor or 
something'; Russian ''tancy-shmancy'' 'dances and similar 
amusements') distinguished from imitative CCs. BW does not consider 
echo-words to be CCs because they have only one lexical slot, the 
second (echo) part being generated by a common phonological rule. 
Meanwhile, the imitative part of an imitative CC is by definition 
meaningless and does not occur independently, so it has hardly more 
rights than an 'echo' to occupy a lexical slot. The only synchronic 
difference here seems to lie in the regularity of the phonological 
correspondences between the meaningful and the meaningless parts, 
which is not a clear-cut distinction; hence a continuous scale between 
the two types. It appears, then, that both types can equally be 
considered or not considered (non-basic) co-compounds.

In some cases, the terminology used might be reconsidered. For 
instance, in Chapter 5, BW uses Cruse's (1986) terms to name the 
compounds like ''husband.PL-wife.PL'' 'husband and wife' converses 
because ''A is the reference point for B and B for A''; he then states 
that compounds for bipartite tools such as 'bow and arrows' ''are also 
a kind of converse, where A and B are each other's reference points''. 
This extension of the term ''converse'' does not seem very 
appropriate. While a husband cannot be a husband of no one, but 
always and only of his wife or wives, a bow does not require an arrow 
to be a bow. Here we find another type of relations otherwise known 
as complementer-complemented relations (Russian ''dopolnitel'-
dopolniaemoe'', cf. Voroncova & Rakhilina 1998); in these relations, 
the two parts do not form a whole but they are necessary for each 
other to function properly.

A remark is to be made on the book's presentation. Due to the 
significance of hyphens for the subject of the study and to their 
frequency in the cited examples, BW keeps hyphens as an 
orthographic means to separate parts of co-compounds, and uses 
dots instead as a morpheme separator, e. g. (Erzya Mordvin ex. 5, p. 
144; palatalization is marked with apostrophe):
<pre>
Nockovt.n'.i.t'-tark.s'.i.t'       ej.se.st
tug.FREQ.PRS.3PL-pull.FREQ.PRS.3PL PP.INESS.POSS3PL
</pre>
This usage seems somewhat confusing and not very readable, as it 
deviates from the widely accepted use of the hyphen as a morpheme 
delimiter, at the same time running into conflict with the use of dot to 
denote cumulation in the glossing line; see also the ''Leipzig glossing 
rules'' in http://www.eva.mpg.de/lingua/files/morpheme.html. (BW 
sometimes also uses colon in glosses, presumably to denote 
cumulation, but never mentions its value explicitly.) I would instead 
suggest replacing the ''compounding'' hyphens with a special symbol, 
such as underscore.

The book is not free from a small number of misprints and slips which, 
in rare cases, complicate the interpretation of the text; cf. in 
5.4.8 'Conclusions' on page 170, ''Echo-words and affirmative-
negative co-compounds occur with the same or often similar functions 
as co-compounds'' instead of ''...affirmative-negative compounds...''.

To summarize, the research presented in the book is an original work 
of high interest to many specialists. One could also mention that BW's 
Ph.D. dissertation, which was the basis for this published study, was 
awarded the 2005 ALT Junior Prize (see 
http://www.lancs.ac.uk/fss/organisations/alt/news/05-36.HTM#sec1), 
although he declined it.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

I am grateful to Natalia Shibasova (Institute of Linguistics, Russian 
Academy of Sciences) for her remarks on a draft of this review.

REFERENCES

Cruse, D. A. (1986) Lexical Semantics. Cambridge: CUP.

Kemmer, Suzanne. (1993) The middle voice. Amsterdam, 
Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Mithun, Marianne. (1984) The evolution of noun incorporation. 
Language 60, pp. 847-895.

Voroncova M. I.; Rakhilina E. V. (1994) Physical object nouns and 
prepositional constructions. Znak: A collection of papers in linguistics, 
semiotics and poetics in commemoration of A. N. Zhurinskij. 
[Predmetnye imena i predlozhnye konstrukcii. Znak: Sbornik statei po 
lingvistike, semiotike i poetike pamyati A. N. Zhurinskogo.] Moscow: 
Russkij uchebnyj centr MS, 1994, pp. 181-190. 

ABOUT THE REVIEWER

Alexandre Arkhipov is a researcher at Lomonosov State University of 
Moscow. His Ph.D. thesis (2005) was devoted to the typology of 
comitative constructions. His research interests also include Basque 
and Daghestanian linguistics, fieldwork and electronic resources for 
field linguists, and semantics of prosody.





-----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-17-798	

	



More information about the LINGUIST mailing list