23.2840, Review: Syntax: De Belder (2011)

linguist at linguistlist.org linguist at linguistlist.org
Tue Jun 26 14:52:16 UTC 2012


LINGUIST List: Vol-23-2840. Tue Jun 26 2012. ISSN: 1069 - 4875.

Subject: 23.2840, Review: Syntax: De Belder (2011)

Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Eastern Michigan U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
            Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>

Reviews: Veronika Drake, U of Wisconsin Madison
Monica Macaulay, U of Wisconsin Madison
Rajiv Rao, U of Wisconsin Madison
Joseph Salmons, U of Wisconsin Madison
Anja Wanner, U of Wisconsin Madison
       <reviews at linguistlist.org>

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org

Do you want to donate to LINGUIST without spending an extra penny? Bookmark
the Amazon link for your country below; then use it whenever you buy from
Amazon!

USA: http://www.amazon.com/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlist-20
Britain: http://www.amazon.co.uk/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlist-21
Germany: http://www.amazon.de/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlistd-21
Japan: http://www.amazon.co.jp/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlist-22
Canada: http://www.amazon.ca/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlistc-20
France: http://www.amazon.fr/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlistf-21

For more information on the LINGUIST Amazon store please visit our
FAQ at http://linguistlist.org/amazon-faq.cfm.

Editor for this issue: Rajiv Rao <rajiv at linguistlist.org>
================================================================  


Date: Tue, 26 Jun 2012 10:51:33
From: Mercedes Tubino-Blanco [mtubino at gmail.com]
Subject: Roots and Affixes

E-mail this message to a friend:
http://linguistlist.org/issues/emailmessage/verification.cfm?iss=23-2840.html&submissionid=4548824&topicid=9&msgnumber=1
 
Discuss this message: 
http://linguistlist.org/pubs/reviews/get-review.cfm?subid=4548824


Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/22/22-4999.html 

AUTHOR: De Belder, Marijke
TITLE: Roots and Affixes
SUBTITLE: Eliminating Lexical Categories from Syntax
SERIES: LOT dissertation series
YEAR: 2011
PUBLISHER: Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics / Landelijke -- LOT

Mercedes Tubino, Department of English, University of Seville (Spain)

SUMMARY

This monograph develops a new syntactic/morphological model that stems from the
revision of the two main current models, Distributed Morphology (DM, Halle &
Marantz 1993) and the Exo Skeletal Model (Borer 2003). Like its two
predecessors, this is a Late Insertion Morphological model that assumes that
lexical material enters any derivation only after the narrow syntax part of the
derivation is concluded. De Belder's doctoral thesis goes a step further in
separating the functional from the lexical part of any derivation in that it:
(i) eliminates categorial heads (n, v, a) from syntax under the assumption that
lexical categories are not language primitives; (ii) defends a view in which
what corresponds with lexical material (known as Root Terminal Nodes in DM
(Harley 2009) and l-morphemes (Harley & Noyer 1999, 2000)) is empty of any
content in syntax. 

Chapter 1 introduces the book's research questions as well as the theoretical
and empirical basis assumed throughout the monograph. De Belder first raises the
double question of whether features of lexical vocabulary items (VIs) and
categorial heads -- two elements generally assumed in DM -- are really active in
narrow syntax. The aim of her thesis is to answer this question negatively. She
argues that lexical vocabulary items cannot contain features linking them with
particular syntactic contexts. For instance, she gives the example of the VI
'dog', which she claims cannot contain such features due to its count and
non-count uses: (2) Three dogs (count use, p. 20) vs. (3) There is dog in the
soup (non-count use, p. 20). She claims that, while both examples involve the
same VI, 'dog', the syntactic context is what determines its meaning as count or
non-count. On the other hand, she argues that categorial functional heads do not
exist in syntax, contra other approaches such as DM. She claims that
derivational affixes, many of which are traditionally seen as the phonetic
realization of categorial positions, are in fact instantiations of Root Terminal
Nodes. For De Belder, the categorial interpretation of words as nouns, verbs or
adjectives is a by-product of functional structure dominating roots, not the
result of categorial heads merged above such roots. For this author, there is a
fundamental difference between lexical and functional material; only the former
is malleable and subject to coercion. She will base many of her contrasts on
this fact. 

In Chapter 2, De Belder develops a theory of the syntax of roots, which is a
revision of previous late insertion accounts. This chapter is the result of her
joint work with Jeroen van Craenenbroeck (2011). She adopts Borer's view that
roots are void of syntactic features, so they cannot select for particular
categories (contra DM's approaches such as Harley & Noyer 2000). De Belder
defends the view that roots are defined structurally and that all VIs, including
those corresponding to root positions, are subject to competition (e.g. they are
all subject to the Subset Principle, as in Halle 1997), although in the case of
lexical VIs, their insertion finally occurs by free choice. This differs from
both traditional and revised DM approaches. For instance, Halle & Marantz (1993)
claim that only VIs that are inserted in functional positions are subject to
competition, whereas the insertion in root positions is done by free choice.
Recent DM approaches (e.g. Siddiqi 2009) argue in favor of competition of VIs to
occupy root positions, but they also assume that lexical VIs contain features.
De Belder rejects these accounts on these grounds, although she adopts a late
(as opposed to early) insertion approach. In the first part of this chapter, she
argues that only a late insertion approach to morphology can explain why we may
find functional VIs occupying root positions, e.g., (7) Martha is mijn tweede
ik, 'Martha is my best friend, lit. Martha is my second I' (p. 42). These
examples also show that roots are defined structurally and not because of their
inherent properties (e.g. the first person pronoun, ik 'I', does not carry
person features with it when used as a root). In the second part of the chapter,
De Belder puts forward the syntactic model that will be used throughout the
book, which involves a revision of previous syntactic models on Merge (Chomsky
2000 and subsequent work). She follows Zwart (2009) in noticing that previous
accounts on Merge involve an asymmetry; whereas primary Merge involves selecting
two elements from numeration at once, subsequent Merge operations involve
selecting one object at once. She proposes a system based on Unary Merge in
which Primary Merge consists of one object selected from Numeration that is
combined with an empty derivation (i.e. the empty set). She argues that this
empty syntactic position is what corresponds with root positions. This implies
that each root in a syntactic structure implies a new derivation, although the
specific details of this mechanism are not discussed until Chapter 5. In the
last part of the chapter, De Belder proposes a modification of the Subset
Principle to ensure the insertion of functional VIs in root positions through
competition. This happens if we assume that the phonological exponent of a VI
results from multiplying the featural content of the syntactic position it will
occupy by the features the VI contains  (any multiplication by zero will void
the featural content of the VI, so any features originally contained in the
functional VI will not block their insertion in root nodes). She finishes the
chapter by addressing potential problems and other accounts (e.g. Harley 2009;
Pfau 2009) she terms as 'late insertion approaches with a limited type of early
insertion'. 

In Chapter 3 De Belder shows that functional structure is sufficient to define
categories. She first presents a case study on the countability of nouns to show
that fine-grained distinctions in the nominal domain are determined
syntactically. That is, she shows that the fact that the same VI (e.g. water)
may be used as count and non-count is the result of syntactic features and their
corresponding functional projections rather than inherent properties of the VIs.
The relevant syntactic features accounting for the different readings of nouns
are [Num] and [Size]. Thus, a VI receives a 'mass' reading if it is inserted in
a syntactic position void of either feature; a VI receives a 'kind' reading if
[Num] is contained in its insertion site, and if both [Num] and [Size] are
contained in its insertion site, then a VI receives a 'unit' reading. She also
argues that a syntactic position containing [Size] only is not possible.
Possible restrictions associated with a count or non-count interpretation of
some VIs are placed on extralinguistic (e.g. conceptual) knowledge. In the
second part of the chapter, De Belder discusses the case of semi-lexical VIs
that may have both a functional and a lexical use, such as the Dutch word 'heel'
('whole'), which may function as both an adjective and a universal quantifier.
She argues that their different denotation is derived from the terminal nodes
this VI may realize as the complement of an adjectival projection, if it is an
adjective. As a quantifier, it originates in Sizeº, moves up to Numº and ends up
in Dº. As a lexical VI, it gets its interpretation entirely from the
Encyclopedia (their denotation is learned and arbitrary), and as a functional
VI, it gets its interpretation from the functional features it realizes in syntax.

In Chapter 4, De Belder discusses why categorial heads do not exist as
primitives of language. Derivational affixes have been long assumed to realize
categorial heads. De Belder, however, discusses the possibility that they
realize 'something else' and argues that her approach is more advantageous,
especially when it comes to affixes associated with more than one category. In
the first part of the chapter, De Belder puts forward a theory of homonyms,
which she identifies as affixes with the same phonetic realization that are
listed as separate vocabulary items. She presents three empirical tests to
distinguish between homonyms and different instances of the same affix based on
allomorphy, based on whether the different instances of the affix have the same
synonyms and co-occurrence. She illustrates the section with the Dutch suffix
-er, which she concludes is the same affix when it is a deverbal and a denominal
agentive suffix, but not in the case of pluralctional -er, which is a case of
homonymy. In the second part of the chapter, De Belder argues that
multicategorial affixes cannot be the realization of categories, hence
categories are not primitives of grammar. She argues that the fact that her
model does not rule out examples such as '*to ugliness' is not a problem for the
claim that categories do not exist in grammar because approaches that assume
categorial heads have the same problem, since the conversion mechanism that
explains cases such as 'to proposition', a derived noun used as a verb, also
fails to rule out '*to ugliness'. She concludes that her model is advantageous
because even though both proposals are equally inadequate in explaining the
ungrammaticality of '*to ugliness', hers is simpler by doing away with
mechanisms such as conversion.

In Chapter 5, De Belder provides an alternative approach to derivational word
formation. She argues that since the meaning of derivational affixes is lexical,
they realize lexical positions (i.e. they realize roots), but like other VIs
that realize root nodes, derivational affixes may be semi-lexical too (i.e.
lexical VIs that realize functional nodes). She presents the case study of Dutch
collective mass nouns (e.g. ondergoed 'underwear') to illustrate this point. She
argues that these nouns are derivational word forms that resist plural (a sign
that they realize functional positions), whereas their collective reading is
derived from the lexical meaning of the affixes. In this chapter, De Belder also
proposes that derived words are concatenated root nodes, where the base VI
realizes the lowest root node and the affixes realize the highest ones.
According to De Belder, root nodes are defined by the absence of any syntactic
features (they are empty derivations that are merged with objects from
Numeration in Unary Merge fashion). Since two concatenated root nodes cannot
result from this mechanism, De Belder proposes that words are the result of (i)
Merge of an object (F, according to De Belder) from Numeration with the empty
set/derivation; (ii) The newly merged complex object is returned to Numeration;
(iii) Merge of the complex object from Numeration with the empty set/derivation,
and so on. Each instantiation of the empty set in a derivation will correspond
to a 'lexical' VI. The remainder of the chapter consists of different
instantiations of the model.

In Chapter 6, the author discusses the theoretical repercussions of her proposed
model and briefly addresses further issues associated with her proposal. For
instance, she suggests that what we know as lexical categories are in fact
defined by groups of features such as [Size] or [Num], in the case of nouns. She
briefly suggests that this can be extended to verbs, which would be the result
of cognitively recognizing information about time, modality, and aspect in
conjunction with argument introducing projections. Another issue briefly treated
in this chapter is, for instance, the elimination of the distinction between
inflection, derivation, and compounding, which should now be viewed as the
distinction between functional and root terminal nodes. 

EVALUATION

De Belder's monograph makes an outstanding contribution to syntactic and
morphological theory, as well as to the analysis of the Dutch nominal domain,
with plenty of examples illustrating all the issues treated, especially
regarding the derivation of Dutch nominal word-forms. The author raises
important questions regarding many assumptions existent in present morphological
theory and effectively challenges them as she searches for a simplified
apparatus that would more effectively account for morphological processes in
language, such as derivation and the existence of derivational affixes. 

I did have some concerns regarding certain issues, both in content and
presentation. First, the monograph would benefit from a revised organization,
since the reader is constantly referred to later sections for information that
may as well have been discussed all at once. The presentation of the theoretical
model proposed is especially problematic, as De Belder covers some parts in
Chapter 2, and leaves the rest for Chapter 5. Although the reader is referred to
Chapter 5 for further information on how the model works, a full description of
the model right at the beginning would make the proposal clearer and save the
reader some unnecessary questions.

Second, the proposal is supported throughout with interesting and new data as
well as illustrations of different phenomena affecting the nominal domain.
However, this discussion would be stronger if examples were provided from
languages other than Dutch (it just includes a few additional examples from
English). Also, while the proposal defended in this thesis is intended as a
theoretical model for grammar, the discussion is limited to the nominal domain,
while its application to the verbal domain is only suggested, but not addressed,
at the end of the book. 

The model proposes as one of its main points the elimination of categories as
part of grammar. However, traditional cases of nominalization or verbalization
are not specifically, but just vaguely, addressed in the present model, which
may leave the reader wondering how they would be resolved. 

The main problem I see with this model is the fact that the insertion of
Vocabulary Items in root nodes (corresponding with elements void of any
phonological, syntactic or semantic information in this model) is completely
unrestricted, as the author herself admits. She suggests that the restriction on
VIs is to be placed on extralinguistic/conceptual knowledge and convention, an
explanation that seems unsatisfactory; while the concept associated with
Vocabulary Items is clearly extralinguistic, Vocabulary Items themselves are
linguistic representations. Their association with particular syntactic
positions needs to be restricted somehow.

Finally, the monograph would have benefited from more careful proof-reading, as
the text contains a considerable number of misspellings that may be distracting
to the reader. 

In sum, De Belder makes an excellent contribution to syntactic and morphological
theory, challenging many ideas widely assumed (i.e. the existence of syntactic
categories as a primitive of grammar). She effectively challenges these
assumptions by showing the reader that a simplified model may be equally
explanatory, hence more advantageous than other models that need to resort to
more complicated apparatus to explain the same phenomena. 

REFERENCES

Borer, Hagit. 2003. Exo-Skeletal vs. endo-skeletal explanations. In John Moore
and Maria Polinsky (eds.), The Nature of Explanation in Linguistic Theory.
Chicago: CSLI and University of Chicago Press. 

Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: the framework. In Roger Martin, David
Michaels and Juan Uriagereka (eds.), Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax
in Honor of Howard Lasnik. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

De Belder, Marijke, & Jeroen van Craenenbroeck. 2011. How to merge a root. Ms., 
HUBrussel & Utrecht University.

Halle, Morris, & Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the pieces of
inflection. In Kenneth Hale and Samuel Jay Keyser (eds.), The view from building
20. Cambridge: MIT Press, 111-176.

Harley, Heidi. 2009. Roots: Identity, Insertion, Idiosyncracies. Talk presented
at the Root Bound workshop, USC, February 21, 2009.

Harley, Heidi and Rolf Noyer. 1998. State-of-the-Article: Distributed
Morphology. Glot International 4.4: 3-9.

Harley, Heidi and Rolf Noyer. 2000. Formal versus encyclopedic properties of
vocabulary. Evidence from nominalisations. In B. Peters (ed.), The
Lexicon-Encyclopedia Interface. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 349-374.

Pfau, Roland. 2009. Grammar as processor. A Distributed Morphology account of
spontaneous speech errors. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 

Siddiqi, Daniel. 2009. Syntax within the word: economy, allomorphy and argument
selection in Distributed Morphology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 

ABOUT THE REVIEWER 

Mercedes Tubino has a PhD in Linguistics from the University of Arizona.
She presently teaches English at the University of Seville (Spain). Her
primary research interests include syntactic theory, morphology,
lexico-semantics, Second Language Acquisition, and Hispanic and Amerindian
linguistics. 






----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-23-2840	
----------------------------------------------------------



More information about the LINGUIST mailing list