24.1461, Review: Sociolinguistics: McWhorter (2011)

linguist at linguistlist.org linguist at linguistlist.org
Sun Mar 31 21:42:53 UTC 2013


LINGUIST List: Vol-24-1461. Sun Mar 31 2013. ISSN: 1069 - 4875.

Subject: 24.1461, Review: Sociolinguistics: McWhorter (2011)

Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Eastern Michigan U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
            Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>

Reviews: Veronika Drake, U of Wisconsin Madison
Monica Macaulay, U of Wisconsin Madison
Rajiv Rao, U of Wisconsin Madison
Joseph Salmons, U of Wisconsin Madison
Anja Wanner, U of Wisconsin Madison
       <reviews at linguistlist.org>

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org

Do you want to donate to LINGUIST without spending an extra penny? Bookmark
the Amazon link for your country below; then use it whenever you buy from
Amazon!

USA: http://www.amazon.com/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlist-20
Britain: http://www.amazon.co.uk/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlist-21
Germany: http://www.amazon.de/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlistd-21
Japan: http://www.amazon.co.jp/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlist-22
Canada: http://www.amazon.ca/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlistc-20
France: http://www.amazon.fr/?_encoding=UTF8&tag=linguistlistf-21

For more information on the LINGUIST Amazon store please visit our
FAQ at http://linguistlist.org/amazon-faq.cfm.

Editor for this issue: Joseph Salmons <jsalmons at linguistlist.org>
================================================================  


Date: Sun, 31 Mar 2013 17:41:01
From: Mauro Giuffre [maurpurgo at hotmail.com]
Subject: Linguistic simplicity and complexity

E-mail this message to a friend:
http://linguistlist.org/issues/emailmessage/verification.cfm?iss=24-1461.html&submissionid=10438241&topicid=9&msgnumber=1
 
Discuss this message: 
http://linguistlist.org/pubs/reviews/get-review.cfm?subid=10438241

Book announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/22/22-2596.html

AUTHOR: McWhorter, John H.
TITLE: Linguistic simplicity and complexity
SUBTITLE: Why do languages undress?
SERIES TITLE: Language contact and bilingualism, Vol.1
PUBLISHER: De Gruyter Mouton
YEAR: 2011

Mauro Giuffrè, Dipartimento di Scienze Filologiche e Linguistiche,
Italia-Sicilia-Università di Palermo

SUMMARY
John McWhorter's Linguistic Simplicity and Complexity is the first volume in
the series “Language contact and bilingualism” and the main topics are
linguistic complexity, second language acquisition and languages in contact.

In McWhorter's earlier anthology, Defining Creole (2005), his view was that
creole languages were definable not just socio-historically, but
grammatically. In Linguistic Simplicity and Complexity, John McWhorter gathers
articles he has written as responses to a wide range of creolists and
linguists and, as noted in the Acknowledgements, he uses them as the basis for
the nine chapters of this volume. They represent a considerable divergence in
direction from his earlier works.

The volume is divided into three sections (Creole exceptionalism, Creole
complexity and Exceptional language change elsewhere) and it opens with an
important Introduction (“The creole litmus test and the NCSL challenge”)
outlining the basic arguments.

The basic proposition is that the difference in complexity between grammars is
determined significantly by the extent to which second-language acquisition
has played a role in their histories. A corollary is that grammatical
simplification can be as significant in languages born of second-language
acquisition as grammatical mixture. That is, descriptions of creolization and
other instances of language mixture as simple “feature selection” from the
languages in a context are incomplete. According to McWhorter’s framework, the
idea that any apparent simplification in a mixed language is always due to the
selection of the least marked choice among the “features” available is
similarly incomplete. There are cases, of which creoles are a type, in which
the choices made to encode most features are less “marked” than any that the
source languages offer. In other words, the guiding principle is
simplification.

>From McWhorter’s point of view, the framework for the analysis of creoles
consists of three main planks: 1) the normal state of language is highly
complex, to the extent that it can seem extreme to speakers of languages like
English. The natural state of human language is extensive marking of fine
semantic and syntactic distinctions, plus rampant allomorphy and irregularity
(typical of languages such as those of the Caucasus and Native America); 2)
languages significantly low in this kind of complexity compared to their
sisters owe this condition to second-language acquisition in the past. This
proposition seems to indicate that under ordinary conditions, languages lose a
modest amount of complexity but maintain a basic level through the development
of new complexities. In this perspective, languages that have not maintained
this level have always been interrupted in their normal accumulation of
complexity (e.g. English, Romance languages, Persian, Mandarin Chinese, and
Indonesian -- McWhorter defines languages of this kind as Non-Hybrid
Conventionalized Second-Language Varieties (NCSLs); 3) the languages which
show the least complexity of all the languages in the world are creoles. The
author considers creoles to be considerably less complex than older languages,
including NCSLs.

McWhorter argues that creole languages are synchronically identifiable, even
without recourse to information about their histories. That is, a creole is “a
kind of language”, not just a sociohistorical term. McWhorter stipulates that
a subset of languages showing the following three patterns were born recently
from pidgins: 1) phonological: little or no use of tone to distinguish
monosyllables or grammatical categories; 2) morphosyntactic: little or no
inflectional morphology; 3) semantic: few or no non-compositional combinations
of derivational markers and roots. Following this perspective, the author
demonstrates that the Creole Prototype constitutes a litmus test for
creolization, with an examination of an isolating language that has not been
considered a creole under any other definition. This is a kind of language
that could be seen as suggesting that even older languages can wend into a
creole-like typology, and that there is therefore properly not a synchronic
essence that is limited only to creole languages. The whole volume is intended
as a demonstration of these theoretical principles.

The first section, Creole exceptionalism, asks whether creolists have been
able to disprove that creole languages harbor evidence of their birth as
pidgins. In the three chapters of the first section, he argues that they have
not and attempts to show that his Prototype and Complexity hypotheses have not
yet been disconfirmed by others scholars in their substance.

Chapter One reviews the Prototype hypothesis, as put forth in McWhorter (2005:
9-37). The Prototype hypothesis is here recast to refute the widely-held
opinion that a Creole is a mixture of two or more languages and that the
result of an unbroken grammar-internal development with merely moderate
abbreviations deriving from adult second-language acquisition. By recasting
the Prototype hypothesis McWhorter affirms that a natural language is a creole
(i.e. recently born from a pidgin) if it has: a) morphologically, little or no
inflectional affixation, and among unbound inflectional markers, no contextual
inflection, or inherent inflection of the paradigmatically complex sort; b)
phonologically, little or no distinction of monosyllabic lexical items or
morphosyntactic distinctions via tone or register, and without any
typologically unusual proliferation of vowels; c) semantically, few or no
noncompositional combinations of nonreduplicative derivational morphemes with
roots. In his opinion, creoles are languages which emerged from structurally
reduced pidgins, and bear these hallmarks of that origin in their current
structural makeup.

Chapter Two shows that the nature of creolization cannot be described in
binary terms. The author claims that creoles should be analyzed as a whole and
that the degree of complexity has to be considered generally. Creoles, as
natural languages, have phonology and morphology, even if those are less
elaborate than those of older languages. This point of view suggests that
creoles are not just hybrids, but that they are human languages that were
reborn only a few centuries ago.

Chapter Three contains a “scientific standoff” with Ansaldo, Matthews and
Lim’s Deconstructing Creole (2007), which denies that the creoles are
identifiable as a synchronic class. In McWhorter’s opinion Deconstructing
Creole paradoxically seems to contain more arguments in favor of the
exceptionalist conception of creoles than against it and reaches the goal of
demonstrating the unexpected conclusion that creoles are the product of
grammatical deconstruction.

The second section (Creole Complexity) contains three chapters, intended to
demonstrate that creole exceptionalism is not based on ignorance of grammar.
McWhorter examines three grammatical features of Saramaccan (spoken in the
Surinamese rain forest, with a largely English and Portuguese lexicon). The
intention is to show that it is not sufficient, when demonstrating that a
creole “has complexity”, to simply indicate that its determiners have some
conditioned allomorphs, that it has derivational processes, or that its
phonology requires rule ordering. The author finds that some features manifest
a more complex rendition of a given construction or semantic concept than in
English or related languages.

Chapter Four expounds the thesis that complexity in creoles grows over time
from an original state. The growth process has occurred over a few centuries
rather than millennia and so the complexities in creoles are not maximally
elaborated in comparison to equivalent features in ‘older’ languages. The
Saramaccan data in this chapter support this view, taking into account some of
the new information marking in Saramaccan. This process surpasses in
overspecification any overt, regularized mechanism in any Western European
language.

The fifth chapter is a re-elaboration of McWhorter (2005: 102-41), where he
discussed creole grammar as a whole. Here he examines two serial verb
constructions encoding movement in the Saramaccan creole and tries to shed
light on the following themes: creoles’ relative complexity compared to older
languages and the possible inadequate description of the grammar of creoles.
McWhorter shows that a construction typically lumped into a list of “serial
verb constructions” in Surinam creole is actually at the heart of a more
complex system for indicating direction than in Western European languages,
where they are conditioned according to transitivity.

In Chapter Six McWhorter supports his scenario for creole genesis, that they
were born as structurally reduced pidginized varieties, eventually expanding
into new natural languages. Analyzing the full range of allomorphy of the
Saramaccan copula, he suggests that grammatical complexity consists of a great
deal more than inflectional affixation and its consequences. The Saramaccan
creole has different levels of complexity in comparison to analytic languages,
not just to Western European languages. The bipartite Saramaccan copula
scenario occurred as the result of explainable but unpredictable processes of
change. Saramaccan speakers have not processed the language as a variety of
any European language, and thus its development of copulas is not couched in a
dynamic relationship with a high superstratal language.

Finally the third section (Exceptional language change elsewhere) is composed
by 3 chapters and represents extensions of the Author’s position on language
contact and simplification to languages other than creoles. “Creole
exceptionalism is but one component of a framework devoted not to the singling
out of creoles for suspect or peculiar reasons, but of analyzing the
difference between human language as it presumably was for the most of humans’
existence, as opposed to the way many languages are now as the result of
population movements and second-language acquisition.” (McWhorter, 2011: 203).

Chapter Seven is adapted from McWhorter (2008), with a reconceived text flow,
additional data and abridgment. This chapter was originally written in
response to Gil (2001), whose article constituted the most challenging reply
to creole exceptionalism. Gil showed that Riau Indonesian was proof that a
language could wend into this “prototypical” state by chance. McWhorter (2008)
concluded that Riau Indonesian is a product of extensive adult acquisition,
and therefore supported the Creole Prototype hypothesis. Gil noted that the
colloquial Indonesian languages were almost all radically less complex than
the standard. Given that Indonesian has been used as much if not more as a
second language than as a first language for over two millennia, McWhorter
thinks that it would be almost bizarre if the nature of these colloquial
dialects was not in some way related to this fact. In seeking to make this
case for dialects like Riau Indonesian, the author surveyed some Austronesian
languages to confirm his hunch that there were no other languages in the
family that had drifted mysteriously into analyticity, but his hunch was
wrong. So McWhorter’s suggestion is that there must be something particular
about the social histories of the Austronesian languages. These languages
therefore present a challenge to the Creole Prototype hypothesis.

Chapter Eight outlines the author’s conclusion concerning the languages of
Flores and Timor; according to him they can be accommodated within his
theoretical proposal. The lexicon of these languages presents some extremely
unusual aspects, considered evidence of a massive interruption that impacted
adult learners.

Chapter Nine is an analysis of English and makes the case that English is even
less “normal” than specialists in its history have typically supposed. In the
author’s opinion English grammar was deeply impacted by Celtic, so that
depending on where one draws the proverbial line, it could be seen as both
moderately simplified as well as moderately mixed. The Celtic Hypothesis on
English diachrony would appear to be at a tipping point. Chapter Nine is
intended as a spur for further study. As language change goes, the path taken
by English, like that of creoles, has been exceptional because of the vast
streamlining and hybridization of its grammar.

EVALUATION
The author assigns himself the task of testing the validity of his basic
proposal, which is that “the difference in complexity between languages’
grammars is determined significantly by the extent to which second-language
acquisition has played a role in their histories”. Each chapter presents a
series of “technical” aspects related to observations that the author links to
the individual languages under consideration. On that front, the author’s
prose is passionate and “appealing”, much more than a standard scientific
book, and the method of investigation of the individual issues covered in the
chapters of the book is rigorous and carefully applied. I reserve judgment on
the conclusions reached in the individual sections to specialists in creoles,
second language acquisition and linguistic contact. I leave to them the value
judgments of a book that for me was interesting to read, one I practically
devoured.

Although they are the target audience of this publication, the title --
Linguistic simplicity and complexity -- aims to reach an audience of scholars
of general linguistics. I belong to this group, with a perspective linked more
to the traditional European study of classical philology. From that
perspective I add a few observations on the project.

First, the use of concepts like “linguistic simplicity” and “complexity” seems
to have a certain pre-scientific quality (i.e. unscientific or unproven
prejudice). I see in his categorization an implicit gradation of value (simple
= of little value; complex = not possessing a general harmony; intermediate =
good, balanced), which becomes strange when we consider that in the category
of the good and balanced he places English and the ‘normal’ Indo-European
languages (all of which have, however, an aurea mediocritas). I detect the
scent of “Eurocentrism”.

Second, the author points out that all linguists “are comfortable with the
basic association between second language acquisition and analyticity as a
tendency. Fewer, however, are comfortable assigning the general typology of a
language to widespread second-language acquisition, in view of the fact that
affixes can erode over time and there is, certainly, a chance element in
complexity differentials between languages.” Though I belong to that smaller
group -- those who can agree to link the general type of a language to its
widespread acquisition as a second language -- I cannot accept that the
analysis of creoles is reduced and confined to the mere perspective of
synchronic analysis, with a general disregard for historical and social
factors (which seem crucial for creoles in particular). Such an analysis seems
to underestimate certain factors in order to insist on the construction of an
analytical scheme composed of various boxes.

Third, the author’s programmatic desire to “work towards a typology of
language contact with not only descriptive but deductive power” causes me
serious discomfort. In linguistics there is no predictive ability that could
be refined to the point of eliminating with a single stroke of a pen the need
to investigate phenomena such as creolization from a complex perspective, one
which integrates synchrony and diachrony and makes it possible to balance
historical and typological factors. Indeed we must recognize that Romance,
Germanic and classical linguistics and their respective 1,500, 1,000 and 3,000
years of scholarly tradition have taught us that this complex perspective is
absolutely essential.

REFERENCES
Ansaldo, Umberto – Matthews, Stephen – Lim, Lisa (eds.) 2007 Deconstructing
Creole. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Gil, David 2001 Creoles, Complexity, and Riau Indonesian. Linguistic Typology
2/3: 325-71.

McWhorter, John 2005 Defining Creole. New York: Oxford University Press.

McWhorter, John 2008 Why does a language undress?: strange cases of Indonesia.
In Miestamo, Matti - Sinnemäki, Kaius - Karlsson, Fred (eds.) 2008 Language
Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Singler, John 2006 Children and creole genesis. Journal of Pidgin and Creole
Languages 21: 157-73.

ABOUT THE REVIEWER
Mauro Giuffré is a post-doc in Linguistics at the University of Palermo. He
holds a PhD in General Linguistics, with a dissertation titled Text
Linguistics and Cognitive Sciences: the proceduralism of Dressler and De
Beaugrande. His main research interests concern the relationship between
classical studies (philology and ancient western European languages, such as
Latin and Greek) and theoretical work in text linguistics; his scientific
production is devoted to connecting theoretical linguistics with classical
studies. He is the editor of STUDIES IN SEMIOTIC TEXTOLOGY IN HONOUR OF JANOS
S. PETÖFI (2011, Supplement 1) (preview in
http://unipa.academia.edu/maurogiuffre/Papers) of Sprachtheorie und
germanistische Linguistik (http://www.sugl.eu/), directed by András Kertész.



----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-24-1461	
----------------------------------------------------------



More information about the LINGUIST mailing list