32.1359, Review: General Linguistics; Linguistic Theories; Syntax: De Clercq (2020)

The LINGUIST List linguist at listserv.linguistlist.org
Fri Apr 16 21:43:42 UTC 2021


LINGUIST List: Vol-32-1359. Fri Apr 16 2021. ISSN: 1069 - 4875.

Subject: 32.1359, Review: General Linguistics; Linguistic Theories; Syntax: De Clercq (2020)

Moderator: Malgorzata E. Cavar (linguist at linguistlist.org)
Student Moderator: Jeremy Coburn, Lauren Perkins
Managing Editor: Becca Morris
Team: Helen Aristar-Dry, Everett Green, Sarah Robinson, Nils Hjortnaes, Joshua Sims, Billy Dickson
Jobs: jobs at linguistlist.org | Conferences: callconf at linguistlist.org | Pubs: pubs at linguistlist.org

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org

Please support the LL editors and operation with a donation at:
           https://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/

Editor for this issue: Jeremy Coburn <jecoburn at linguistlist.org>
================================================================


Date: Fri, 16 Apr 2021 17:43:09
From: Mingya Liu [mingya.liu at hu-berlin.de]
Subject: The Morphosyntax of Negative Markers

 
Discuss this message:
http://linguistlist.org/pubs/reviews/get-review.cfm?subid=36640217


Book announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/31/31-2003.html

AUTHOR: Karen  De Clercq
TITLE: The Morphosyntax of Negative Markers
SUBTITLE: A Nanosyntactic Account
SERIES TITLE: Studies in Generative Grammar [SGG]
PUBLISHER: De Gruyter Mouton
YEAR: 2020

REVIEWER: Mingya Liu, Humboldt University of Berlin

Reviewed by Andreas Blümel and Mingya Liu

SUMMARY

This book by Karen De Clercq (KDC) studies the morphosyntax of canonical
predicate negators such as the negative adverbs “not/n’t” and negative affixes
“non-”, “un-”/“dis-”/“iN-” in English. While these negative markers (NMs) are
usually conceived as belonging to different descriptive domains, KDC develops
a uniform treatment of their internal structure and external distribution
within nanosyntax (NS, Starke (2009) et seq), a framework which extends and
modifies cartography (Rizzi (1997), Cinque (1999), a.o).  The main idea is
that these different NMs in the clausal spine as well as in prefixal function
underlyingly share a universal set of features which differ in “size.” The NMs
each occupy the specifier position of a NegP sitting in different clausal
positions. In the following, we briefly summarize the content of each chapter.

Chapter 1 introduces the book and the main points.

In Chapter 2 KDC meticulously and in a reader-friendly way lays out the tenets
of NS (Starke (2009) et seq), like “late,” i.e. postsyntactic insertion of
Vocabulary Items (“lexical items” in KDC’s terminology), the idea that
features or even values on features are heads of syntactic projections, i.e. a
given syntactic head cannot possibly “bundle” features. The insertion of
Vocabulary Items is determined by three principles (A) cyclic spellout: “after
every application of Merge, spellout is mandatory” (p. 9), (B) the superset
principle which dictates that “A lexically stored tree matches a syntactic
node iff the lexically stored tree contains the syntactic node” (Starke (2009:
3) in De Clercq (2020: 17)) and (C) the Elsewhere Condition (Kiparsky (1973)
in De Clercq (2020: 17)). “Containment” has become another important principle
in — not only —  NS (cf. Caha (2009), Bobaljik (2012)), which assumes
“structural containment relations” (p. 14) between e.g. different case
phrases: nominative is contained in an accusative phrase, accusative phrase is
contained in a genitive phrase, etc.. Syntax is the pivot within this
framework which makes no difference between word formation and sentence
structure. That is, there is one component responsible for all structure
building, instead of separate modules of grammar. 

Chapter 3 deals with the classification of NMs with English data. KDC argues
that the different NMs are carriers of subatomic features T/Neg, Foc/Neg,
Class/Neg and Q/Neg, with Q being the head of a functional projection
responsible for gradability sitting between DegP and AP (Corver (1997). She
backs up her distinction with four criteria. The first two and most crucial
criteria are the stacking and scope properties of these NMs: “The rule is that
the negative marker which can stack on most items take wides scope” (KDC
(2020:34)). KDC proposes the following classification for English: 
T/Neg-markers (“n’t/not”)  for predicate denial, Foc/Neg-makers (“not”) for
predicate negation and two kinds of predicate term negation, namely,
Class/Neg-makers (“non-”) and Q/Neg-markers (“un-/dis-/iN-”). The NegPs
hosting these NMs occupy different syntactic positions.  T/Neg sits highest,
taking scope over TP (no matter whether they precede or follow tense
morphology), giving rise to sentence negation. Foc/Neg sits between TP and
FocP.  Class/Neg sits between vP and ClassP and Q/Neg sits lowest between
ClassP and QP. The syntactic positions mirror the semantic scope of the NMs.
The third and fourth criteria are semantic: KDC relates T-/Foc-/Class-Neg to
contradiction and Q-Neg to contrariety, and assigns the function of denial to
T-Neg, of contrast (or emphasis/ modification) to Foc-Neg, of classification
to Class-Neg and of characterization to Q-Neg.

Chapter 4 surveys data from 23 languages of different language families, with
regard to the morphosyntax of the NMs.  KDC distinguishes seven language
patterns, depending on the degrees of syncretism between the four types of
NMs. Pattern 1 refers to languages that are fully non-syncretic in this
aspect, that is, they use different NMs for T-, Foc-, Class and Q-negation,
including Greek, Korean, informal English and formal French. Pattern 7 refers
to languages that are fully syncretic, that is, they use the same NM for
T/Neg, Foc/Neg, Class/Neg and Q/Neg. Examples are Czech, Hixkaryana (a Carib
language), Russian, Macedonian, Malagasy, and Tümpisa Shoshone. Pattern 2-6
refers to languages that have increasing degrees of syncretism. For instance,
Mandarin Chinese belongs to Pattern 3 for which it is claimed that the NM “bu”
is used for both T/Neg and Foc/Neg and “fei” is used for both Class/Neg and
Q/Neg, sharing the same pattern with Persian, Modern Standard Arabic and
Mayalayam. 

In Chapter 5 the internal structure (“syntax”) of NM is investigated. KDC
seeks to show “how the syncretisms and the nanosyntactic methodology provide
insight into the internal structure of a negative morpheme” (p. 143). KDC
argues that the internal structure of NMs comprises the following four
functional heads, projected from what she considers the “lexical” core Neg:

(1) T/Neg – Foc/Neg – Class/Neg – Q/Neg – Neg

Following nanosyntactic custom, KDC uses the patterns of syncretisms as a
probe into the internal structure of words. The guiding principle is that
syncretic markers have a syntax in which the heads are closer to one another
than the one which underlies non-syncretic NMs:

Universal Negation Contiguity Hypothesis (p. 145): “Negation syncretism
targets contiguous regions of negative markers invariant across languages.”

The observed patterns of syncretism from Chapter 4 lead her to a structure in
which (1) translates into hierarchical structure and is the basis for her
dismissal of an alternative.

Chapter 6 is devoted to showing that the internal structure of NMs in (1)
mirrors the relative hierarchy of functional negation heads within the clausal
spine. (2)/(160) is the structure which shows the full set of functional
negation heads interspersed with a subset of the functional heads which KDC
assumes for a clausal structure:

(2) (Force) (…) T/Neg (T) Foc/Neg (Foc, v) Class/Neg (Class) Q/Neg (Q)

Following the familiar cartographic approach to adverbs by Cinque (1999), KDC
assumes that NMs are XPs which occupy specifier positions of these sentential
Neg-heads. On the “categorical side,” the criterion for inserting these
specifiers is a matching requirement for the features on a given Neg-head in
(1): T/Neg in (1) is inserted above the sentential level T in (2), Foc/Neg in
(1) is inserted at the sentential level Foc in (2), etc.. This entails a
parallelism between the structural complexity of NM and the structural
complexity of the clausal syntax: The higher the NM in the clause, the richer
its featural structure. Following Starke’s (2004) line of reasoning that
specifiers and the heads they occupy are redundant, all the sentential
functional heads in (2) can be eliminated as “specifiers project and provide
the feature required by the FSEQ” (p. 162), i.e. functional sequence.

The discussion on the insertion of negative “PRE-markers” (p. 164) (i.e. NMs
that are head-initial or prefixal) hinges on the question whether or not
Vocabulary Items have “unary bottoms” (vacuous projection) or “binary bottoms”
for technical reasons inherent to NS. 

An algorithm guiding the insertion of Vocabulary Items is given on p. 165
(later, p. 189, extended by yet another movement-inducing instruction
preceding all others):

“335. a. Insert feature and spell out (=do not move)

b. If fail, try a cyclic (spec-to-spec) movement of the node inserted at the
previous cycle

c. If fail, try a snowball movement of the complement of the newly inserted
feature and spell out.

d. If merge-f has failed to spell out (even after backtracking), try to spawn
a new derivation providing feature X and merge that with the current
derivation, projecting feature X to the top node.”

The difference between “unprofessional” and “non-professional” is rooted in
the presence or absence of ClassP, which the former lacks while the latter has
it. Crucially, both words involve the creation of a complex specifier (the
phrases underlying the prefixes “un-” and “non-” respectively) for technical
reasons inherent to NS. Crucial for their insertion, according to (335), is
that the derivation resorts to base-generation/External Merge of a complex XP
(a specifier) if all other options ((335-a) to (335-c)) fail.

KDC subsequently addresses the issue of suppletive forms like /iN-/ or /-dis/
in the English “impatient” or “dishonest”, utilizing the nanosyntactic notion
of pointers (cf. Starke (2014)), which is “a reference in the lexical tree of
a lexical item [...] to another [lexical item]” (p. 173). KDC then analyzes
negative suffixal markers like “-less”, where the “unary bottom” comes into
play (p. 178). The chapter ends with an astonishing 10-page discussion and
analysis of English “n’t”, i.e. the contracted form of negation, which is an
affixal element within KDC’s treatment.

The 10-page Chapter 7 captures diachronic change of NMs in le bon usage French
of the familiar “ne ... pas” kind in terms of a distribution of different
functional features in the clausal spine over the Vocabulary items for “ne”
and “pas”. Since “[o]nly the combination [of these features] yield what is
conceived of as sentential negation,” (p. 194) their obligatory co-occurrence
to express sentential negation follows. In Colloquial French, then, the
featural content of the Vocabulary Item “pas” increases such that it alone is
a candidate for the syntactic environment of sentential negation, rendering
“ne” redundant.

Chapter 8 reviews the existing accounts of postulating multiple NegPs for the
different surface positions of NMs in a clause and presents the differences
between the account in the book with these. In comparison, Zanuttini (1997)
and Poletto (2017) focus on sentential negation in Italian dialects, whereas
the book provides a uniform account of both sentential negation and
morphological negations across languages.

Chapter 9  deals with semantic issues. Even though it is quite short, it is
essential to complement the morphosyntactic account developed in the book. It
shows that the interpretation of negative expressions, with the example of
contradiction vs. contrariety, is not a result of NMs only but of their
interplay with the modified predicates. 

Chapter 10 concludes the book. 

EVALUATION

KDC draws on an impressive range of typologically unrelated and diverse
languages to motivative her feature hierarchy, an important and valuable
descriptive generalization which is a remarkable achievement. She comes up
with creative solutions in the face of recalcitrant data (e.g. the enrichment
of the spellout algorithm on p. 189). While the variation of NMs across
languages as well as within a language poses a challenge for a uniform
morphosyntactic analysis, KDC makes a bold attempt utilizing NS. We believe
that both data and theory in the book contribute to a better understanding of
NMs across languages and provoke discussion. For example, an important
research topic is the relationship between uniform lexical items (understood
as items drawn from the lexicon), the structure underlying word formation and
consequences the two have for the ongoing derivation. KDC’s contribution makes
a stab in this area of research (cf. Epstein, Kitahara & Seely (2016)
addressing similar issues with different assumptions). The book is very
clearly written, and thus is very accessible even for readers who are
unfamiliar with NS. Furthermore, KDC is very explicit in the theoretical
implementation, while showing an awareness of open questions and limitations
at the same time, for example, in the empirical coverage of the proposed
analyses (“The data are restricted to predicate declarative main clauses with
copular verbs and adjectival predicates in the simple present tense.”, p.
231).

While the book focuses on morphosyntax, it touches upon semantic issues
related to NMs and their co-occurring expressions, for example, in the
discussion of the scalar meaning of adjectives in Chapter 9, of register
(Mandarin) and of the lexical origins of the predicates (Korean). This shows
that NMs are sensitive to various properties of the modified predicates,
raising further research questions: How is the meaning of a negative
expression compositionally derived and what role do NMs and their co-occurring
expressions play exactly? What further, possibly universal, factors determine
their combinatory constraints in this aspect, and in which ways?

The book focuses on standard NMs without dealing with n-words, negative
quantifiers, negative polarity items, neg-raising, negative concord, or
pleonastic/expletive negation. The collected cross-linguistic data call for
future investigations.  For example, the data in Mandarin Chinese in Section
4.2.3.1. are confined to a more formal literary variety of the language,
classified as a Pattern 3 language. Whether the classification extends to
modern colloquial Mandarin Chinese is an open question, as for example, “bu”
can be used not only as T/Neg and Foc/Neg but also as Q/Neg, seemingly a
problem for the contiguity required for a successful account of syncretism.

KDC brings up the deep and vexing problem of how specifiers are introduced
into the derivation: “It is somehow assumed that this specifier is assembled
in another workspace and then somehow plugged into the derivation. How this
exactly happens and how these different derivations interact is not usually
explained. [And KDC provides] an algorithm for how this happens.” (p. 161)
While KDC addresses an important topic (but see the technical aspects in
Chomsky et al (2019) where the notion of workspaces is the centerpiece), the
solution she opts for, namely adopting Starke’s (2004) conjecture that
specifiers project (cf. e.g. (333), p. 163) equally begs the question how and
under what circumstances this works, while otherwise traditional
X-bar-theoretic notions like “projection” seem to be adopted. Arguably, this
worry concerns NS more generally, not just KDC’s usage of it. Furthermore, in
our view, the uniformity of disparate syntactic and morphological
NM-phenomena, which the nanosyntactic formalism provides for, might not be
without cost, the computational complexity of the algorithm (335) and the
tacit assumptions hidden in there being one example.

REFERENCES

Bobaljik, J. (2012) “Universals in comparative morphology: Suppletion,
superlatives, and the structure of words”. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Caha, P. (2009) “The nanosyntax of case”. Doctoral thesis, University of
Tromsø.

Chomsky, N., Á. Gallego & D. Ott (2019) “Generative Grammar and the Faculty of
Language: Insights, Questions, and Challenges”. Catalan Journal of
Linguistics, 229-261.

Cinque, G. (1999) “Adverbs and Functional Heads. A Cross-Linguistic
Perspective”. New York: Oxford University Press.

Corver, N. (1997) “Much-Support as a Last Resort”. Linguistic Inquiry 28 (1),
119-164.

Epstein, S., H. Kitahara & D. Seely (2016) “Phase cancellation by external
pair-merge of heads”. The Linguistic Review 33, 87-102.

Kiparsky, P. (1973) ““Elsewhere” in Phonology”. In: S. Anderson & P. Kiparsky 
(eds.) A Festschrift for Morris Halle, 93-106. New York: Holt, Rinehait and
Winston.

Poletto, C. (2017) “Negative doubling: in favor of a big NegP analysis”. In:
S. Cruschina, K. Hartmann & E.-M. Remberger (eds.), Studies on negation:
syntax, semantics, and variation, 81-104. Göttingen: Vandenhoek & Rupprecht.

Rizzi, L. (1997) “The Fine Structure of the Left Periphery”. In:  L. Haegeman
(ed.) Elements of Grammar: A Handbook of Generative Syntax, 281-337.
Dordrecht: Kluwer. 

Starke, M. (2004) “On the inexistence of specifiers and the nature of heads”.
In: A. Belletti (ed.), The Cartography of Syntactic Structures, vol.
3:Structures and Beyond, 252-268. New York: Oxford University Press.

Starke, M. (2009) “Nanosyntax: A short primer to a new approach to language”.
In: Nordlyd, 36(1), 1-6.

Starke, M. (2014) “Cleaning up the lexicon”. Linguistic Analysis 39, 245-256.

Zanuttini, R. (1997) “Negation and clausal structure: a comparative study of
Romance languages”. New York: Oxford University Press.


ABOUT THE REVIEWER

Please note that the book is reviewed by TWO authors: Andreas Blümel
(University of Göttingen) and Mingya Liu (Humboldt University of Berlin)

Andreas Blümel is a Postdoc in the German Department of the University of
Göttingen. He completed his dissertation 'Propagating Symmetry: Case Studies
in Exocentric Syntax' at the University of Frankfurt am Main in 2014. His
research interests lie in the syntax of German, long distance dependencies,
the syntax of nominal phrases and theoretical syntax more generally.

Mingya Liu is an Assistant Professor in the Department of English and American
Studies of the Humboldt University of Berlin. She completed her dissertation
''Multidimensional Semantics of Evaluative Adverbs“ at the University of
Göttingen in 2012. Her research focuses on formal and experimental semantics
and pragmatics, of a.o., negation and polarity items.





------------------------------------------------------------------------------

***************************    LINGUIST List Support    ***************************
 The 2020 Fund Drive is under way! Please visit https://funddrive.linguistlist.org
  to find out how to donate and check how your university, country or discipline
     ranks in the fund drive challenges. Or go directly to the donation site:
                   https://crowdfunding.iu.edu/the-linguist-list

                        Let's make this a short fund drive!
                Please feel free to share the link to our campaign:
                    https://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/
 


----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-32-1359	
----------------------------------------------------------






More information about the LINGUIST mailing list