[Dgkl] Final CFP: Dissecting Morphological Theory 3
Olga Steriopolo
steriopolo at leibniz-zas.de
Wed Jan 26 12:20:00 UTC 2022
Final CFP: Dissecting Morphological Theory 3: Diminutivization, Allomorphy and the Architecture of Grammar
Workshop to be held in conjunction with the 20th International Morphology Meeting
Budapest, 1-4 September 2022, http://www.nytud.hu/imm20/
Workshop website: https://sites.google.com/view/morphologytheories-diminutives<https://sites.google.com/view/morphologytheories-diminutives/home>
Abstract submission deadline: 15 January 2022 16 February 2022
EasyChair submission link: https://easychair.org/conferences/?conf=dmtd3
Organizers
Stela Manova, University of Vienna, stela.manova at univie.ac.at<mailto:stela.manova at univie.ac.at>
Katharina Korecky-Kröll, University of Vienna, katharina.korecky-kroell at univie.ac.at<mailto:katharina.korecky-kroell at univie.ac.at>
Olga Steriopolo, Leibniz-ZAS Berlin, olgasteriopolo at hotmail.com<mailto:olgasteriopolo at hotmail.com>
Scientific committee
Artemis Alexiadou, Humboldt University & Leibniz-ZAS, Berlin
Mark Aronoff, Stony Brook University, SUNY
Boban Arsenijević, University of Graz
Olivier Bonami, Université de Paris
Pavel Caha, Masaryk University, Brno
Guglielmo Cinque, Ca' Foscari University of Venice
Marijke De Belder, University of Oldenburg
David Embick, University of Pennsylvania
Maria Gouskova, New York University
Laura Grestenberger, Austrian Academy of Sciences, Vienna
Katharina Korecky-Kröll, University of Vienna
Lívia Körtvélyessy, Pavol Jozef Šafárik University, Košice
Stela Manova, University of Vienna
Ora Matushansky, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique & Paris VIII
Olga Steriopolo, Leibniz-ZAS, Berlin
Keren Rice, University of Toronto
Maria Voeikova, Russian Academy of Sciences, St. Petersburg
Martina Wiltschko, ICREA, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona
This workshop is the third of a series of workshops on diminutive morphology and its implications for morphological theory. The workshops are held in conjunction with different international conferences: https://sites.google.com/view/morphologytheories-diminutives<https://sites.google.com/view/morphologytheories-diminutives/home>.
Diminutive morphology presents a number of challenges to morphological theory and various issues have been discussed extensively: whether diminutivization is derivation or inflection (Dressler 1989; Scalise 1988; Stump 1993; Manova 2011; Grandi & Körtvélyessy 2015); are diminutive suffixes heads and/or modifiers (Wiltschko and Steriopolo 2007; Steriopolo 2009, 2015, 2016; Gouskova & Bobaljik, to appear); do they attach “low” or “high” in the syntactic tree (De Belder et al. 2014; Cinque 2015); which meanings are associated with diminutive morphology (Dressler & Merlini Barbaresi 1994; Jurafsky 1996) and so on. Nevertheless, there are still issues that have remained unaddressed:
1. Why do some languages have large sets of diminutive affixes, while others have very limited sets?
2. What is a diminutive allomorph? (Should allomorphs have the same semantic-pragmatic function, e.g. could they have different readings, either positive or negative, depending on the situation? Should allomorphs be associated with the same inflection class? Should allomorphs have the same syntactic function: are they either heads or modifiers or could they be both; could they attach at different “heights” in the syntactic tree, resulting in “high” vs. “low” allomorphs?)
3. How does allomorph selection take place in diminutivization? (Is it based on semantics, on form, on syntactic structure, on linearization, or on extragrammatical information?)
4. Are gender and inflection class encoded in the same way in diminutive and non-diminutive nouns? (If diminutive affixes impose gender and inflection class, what does this mean for our understanding of the morphology-syntax interface?)
5. What architecture of grammar best captures the peculiarities of diminutive morphology?
(a) Phonology after morphology, i.e. morphologically conditioned phonology (and consequently phonology-free syntax)
(b) Phonology before morphology, i.e. phonologically conditioned morphology (and maybe also syntax)
(c) A mixture of (a) and (b).
Human languages can be broadly divided into diminutive-rich and diminutive-poor. Intriguingly, even some of the diminutive-poor languages (e.g. English is of this type) have more than one diminutive affix. As can be expected, diminutive-rich languages (e.g. Slavic and Romance languages) possess extensive sets of diminutive affixes. To illustrate, Bulgarian (Slavic) uses the following suffixes for derivation of diminutive nouns:
1. Nominal diminutive suffixes in Bulgarian (examples in Manova & Winternitz 2011)
-ec, -l(e), -č(e), -k(a), -ic(a), -ičk(a), -čic(a),-c(e), -ic(e), -enc(e), -(e)
With respect to (1), the following research questions arise. First, why does a language need a (large) set of diminutive affixes? And second, are all diminutive affixes phonological and suppletive variants (i.e. allomorphs) or is there an additional motivation for them, e.g. structural, semantic, cognitive, pragmatic, psycholinguistic, sociolinguistic?
Affix allomorphy
In linguistic literature, affix allomorphs are usually defined as variants conditioned by the bases to which they attach. They express the same meaning and occur in complementary distribution. Such definitions do not mention the feature-set specification of allomorphs or their position in the syntactic tree. However, a diminutive variant is not always conditioned by the base, and diminutive affixes are not necessarily in complementary distribution, as shown in (2).
2. No conditioning by the base, Hund ‘dog’ (m.) (German)
a. Hünd-chen (n.), Hund-chen (n.)
b. Hünd-ilein (n.), Hund-ilein (n.) <https://www.wortbedeutung.info/H%C3%BCndlein/>
c. Hünd-lein (n.)
d. Hund-erl (n.)
e. Hund-ili (n.)
f. Hund-i (n.)
In (2a) and (2c), both suffixes -chen and -lein derive Standard German diminutives. Overall, -chen forms are more frequent, while -lein diminutives appear old-fashioned and more typical of literary texts. Nevertheless, in some cases -lein is used instead of -chen, due to phonological restrictions, as in (3).
3. Phonologically conditioned allomorphy: -chen vs -lein (German)
a. Buch (n.) ‘book’ → *Büch-chen, Büch-lein (n.) (*chch)
b. Ball (m.) ‘ball’ → Bäll-chen (n.), *Bäll-lein (*lll)
Allomorph selection can also be conditioned by style and register. For example, Hund-ilein in (2b), Hund-ili in (2e) and Hund-i in (2f) are all child-centered forms. Allomorphy can also be conditioned by sociolinguistic factors, e.g. a dialectal use, as in (2d), Hund-erl is used in Bavarian dialects.
Additionally, if a language has a rich set of diminutive affixes, some of them may be gender-preserving, while others may be gender-changing, as shown in (4) for Bulgarian. Are -ec and -č(e) allomorphs of the same diminutive suffix?
4. Gender-preserving vs gender-changing diminutive suffixes (Bulgarian)
a. glas (m.)‘voice’ → glas-ec (m.) ‘light voice’
b. glas (m.) ‘voice’ → glas-č(e) (n.) ‘light voice’
The issue of diminutive affix allomorphy has been extensively discussed for Russian diminutive nouns. For example, Gouskova and Bobaljik (to appear), contra Bonet & Harbour (2012) for other languages, maintain that the Russian suffix -onok has two variants: the gender- & inflection-class-changing -onok deriving baby diminutives and the gender-preserving inflection-class-changing -onk(a), an evaluative suffix with a dismissive/affectionate flavor. They classify -onok as a head and -onk(a) as a modifier. By contrast, Steriopolo (2009) assigns the status of a syntactic head to all inflection-class-changing diminutive suffixes. Thus, a question arises: Could allomorphs differ in syntactic function / be associated with different sets of morphosyntactic features in theories that do not use the head-modifier distinction?
How should all this be modeled theoretically?
Current morphology is dominated by realizational theories such as Distributed Morphology (DM, Halle & Marantz 1993, Bobaljik 2017, among others) and Paradigm Function Morphology (PFM, Stump 2001, 2016, among others). Such theories treat meaning and form separately, i.e. they assume that morphological derivation first happens at an abstract level (semantics associated with syntactic terminal nodes in DM; content paradigms in PFM) and only afterwards, phonological realizations (vocabulary items) are inserted in DM; forms are linked to content in PFM. In other words, in realizational theories, phonology is postponed. Thus, a question arises: How does a diminutive meaning match its phonological realization, especially when different realizations that seem neither phonologically nor morphologically conditioned are available and/or when there are gaps in the derivational paradigm, such as the ones in (5)?
(5) Derivational paradigm involving diminutive nouns and verbs (German)
a. tanzen/ Tanz ‘to/ dance’ → Tänzchen, Tänzlein, Tanzerl, ?Tänzerl, ?Tänzel, dim. verb: tänzeln
b. buchen/ Buch ‘to/ book’ → *Büchchen (*chch), Büchlein, Bücherl, Büchel, dim. verb: *bücheln
c. kochen/ Koch ‘to/ cook’ → *Köchchen (*chch), ?Köchlein, dim. verb: köcheln
(? - rather potential than actual)
It is important to point out that theories that operate with classical morphemes (e.g. Natural Morphology (Dressler et al. 1987) and Minimalist Morphology (Wunderlich 1996)), i.e. theories that recognize the morpheme as the smallest unit of language structure relating meaning and form have a similar problem with data such as these in (5), i.e. the question remains: How do speakers select a diminutive morpheme?
A diminutive morpheme may impose gender and inflection-class, as in (4) and (6). However, these are different types of features: gender determines agreement classes, while an inflection class is "a set of lexemes whose members each select the same set of inflectional realizations" (Aronoff 1994: 64). Inflection class information is not syntactically motivated but diacritic and it is also not syntactically active at the level of Logical Form (Alexiadou 2004).
(6) The diminutive suffix -chen imposes neuter gender and zero plural (German)
a. [+ gender change,+ inflection class change]
der Ball m. sg. ‘ball’, die Bälle m. pl. (-e + umlaut) → das Bäll-chen n. sg., die Bäll-chen n. zero pl.
b. [+ gender change, – inflection class change]
der Beutel m. sg. ‘bag, pouch’, die Beutel m. zero pl. → das Beutel-chen n. sg., die Beutel-chen n. zero pl.
c. [– gender change, + inflection class change]
das Schiff n. sg. ‘ship’, die Schiffe n. pl. (-e) → das Schiff-chen n. sg., die Schiff-chen n. zero pl.
· Is the gender feature encoded in the diminutive morpheme? If yes, what does this mean for a-morphous theories of morphology (PFM; Word-Based Morphology (WBM), Blevins 2006; Construction Morphology, Booij 2010) where one cannot encode features in morphemes and for syntax-based theories (with abstract morphemes) such as DM, Nanosyntax (Caha 2020) and Cartography (Cinque & Rizzi 2015)? The latter two are one-feature-one-head and do not allow feature clustering (feature clustering is possible in DM).
· Inflection classes are particularly prominent in WBM and PFM. Thus, is inflection-class information in diminutives encoded at the level of the word (WBM), at the level of the stem (PFM) or at the level of the morpheme (DM)? In generative grammar, some scholars consider inflection class a syntactic feature (Steriopolo 2017, Kučerová 2018), while others see it as a post-syntactic phenomenon (Alexiadou & Müller 2008, Embick 2010, Kramer 2015). We especially encourage proposals addressing the relationship between diminutivization and inflection class from both a cross-linguistic and a language-specific perspective.
· Is a diminutive suffix listed in the mental lexicon (and inserted, in the sense of vocabulary insertion) as a complex piece of structure together with the inflection it imposes, that is, as a fixed two-suffix combination (= bigram), cf. Manova & Knell (2021)?
We invite papers that tackle any aspect of diminutive allomorphy within any linguistic theory, including papers on the diachronic development of allomorphy in diminutive morphology. Contributions that analyze not only selected affixes but also complete diminutive systems and/or relate their findings to the architecture of grammar are particularly welcome.
Abstract submission
2-page anonymous abstracts for 20-minute presentations (plus 10 minutes for discussion) should be submitted via EasyChair: https://easychair.org/conferences/?conf=dmtd3. Submissions can be modified in EasyChair until 16 February 2022 (click on “View” and then select the necessary update option).
Submission of the same abstract to both the workshop and the IMM20 main session is not allowed. IMM20 submissions are limited to one individual and one joint abstract (or two joint ones) per person. For additional information on abstract submission for the main session, check the IMM20 website: http://www.nytud.hu/imm20/.
Important dates
Abstract submission deadline: 15 January 2022 16 February 2022
Acceptance notifications: 31 May 2022 (for all sessions of IMM20)
Conference: 1-4 September 2022
References
Alexiadou, Artemis (2004). On the development of possessive determiners: consequences for DP structure. In E. Fuß & C. Trips (eds.) Diachronic Clues to Synchronic Grammar. John Benjamins, 31–58.
Alexiadou, Artemis & Gereon Müller (2008). Class Features as Probes. In A. Bachrach & A. Nevins (eds.) Inflectional Identity, 101–155. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Aronoff, Mark (1994). Morphology by itself. Cambridge, MA: MIT.
Blevins, James P. (2006). Word-based morphology. J. Linguistics 42: 531–573.
Bobaljik, Jonathan (2017). Distributed Morphology. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Retrieved 17 Jun. 2020, https://oxfordre.com/linguistics/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.001.0001/acrefore-9780199384655-e-131.
Bonami, Olivier & Jana Strnadová (2019). Paradigm structure and predictability in derivational morphology. Morphology 29(2): 167–197.
Bonami, Olivier & Gregory Stump (2017). Paradigm Function Morphology. In Hippisley, A. & Stump, G. (eds.) The Cambridge handbook of morphology, 449–481.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Bonet, Eulàlia & Daniel Harbour (2012). Contextual Allomorphy. In J. Trommer (ed.) The Morphology and Phonology of Exponence, 195–235. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Booij, Geert (1996). Inherent versus contextual inflection and the split morphology hypothesis. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (eds.) Yearbook of Morphology 1995, 1–16.
Booij, Geert (2010). Construction Morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Caha, Pavel (2020). Nanosyntax: some key features. Ms., submitted to the Handbook of Distributed Morphology, lingbuzz/004437.
Cinque, Guglielmo (2015). Augmentative, pejorative, diminutive and endearing heads in the extended nominal projection. In E. Di Domenico, C. Hamann & S. Matteini (eds.) Structures, strategies and beyond: Studies in honour of Adriana Belletti, 67–82. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Cinque, Guglielmo & Luigi Rizzi (2015). The Cartography of Syntactic Structures. In B. Heine & H. Narrog (eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Analysis, 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
De Belder, Marijke, Noam Faust & Nicola Lampitelli (2014). On a low and a high diminutive: Evidence from Italian and Hebrew. In A. Alexiadou, H. Borer & Florian Schaefer (eds.) The syntax of roots and the roots of syntax, 149–163. Oxford University Press.
Dressler, Wolfgang U. (1989). Prototypical differences between inflection and derivation. Zeitschrift für Phonetik, Sprachwissenschaft und Kommunikationsforschung 42: 3–10.
Dressler, Wolfgang U. & Katharina Korecky-Kröll (2015). Evaluative morphology and language acquisition. In Nicola Grandi & Livia Körtvélyessy (eds.). Edinburgh Handbook of Evaluative Morphology, 134–141. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Dressler,Wolfgang U., Willi Mayerthaler, Oswald Panagl & Wolfgang U. Wurzel (1987). Leitmotifs in Natural Morphology. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Dressler, Wolfgang U. & Lavinia Merlini Barbaresi (1994). Morphopragmatics: diminutives and intensifiers in Italian, German, and other languages. Berlin: de Gruyter.
Dressler, Wolfgang U., Lavinia Merlini Barbaresi, Sonja Schwaiger, Jutta Ransmayr, Sabine Sommer-Lolei & Katharina Korecky-Kröll (2019). Rivalry and lack of blocking among Italian and German diminutives in adult and child language. In F. Rainer, F. Gardani, W.U. Dressler & H.C. Luschützky (eds.) Competition in Inflection and Word Formation, 123–143. Cham: Springer.
Embick, David (2010). Localism versus Globalism in Morphology and Phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gouskova, Maria & Jonathan D. Bobaljik (to appear). The lexical core of a complex functional affix: Russian baby-diminutive -onok. To appear in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, also lingbuzz/005660
Gouskova, Maria & Jonathan D. Bobaljik (2020). Allomorphy and Vocabulary Insertion, lingbuzz/005388
Grandi, Nicola & Livia Körtvélyessy (eds.). (2015). Edinburgh Handbook of Evaluative Morphology. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Halle, Morris & Alec Marantz (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale & S.J. Keyser (eds.) The view from building 20, 111–176. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Inkelas, Sharon (2014), The interplay of morphology and phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Jurafsky, Daniel (1996). Universal tendencies in the semantics of the diminutive. Language 72(3): 533–577.
Korecky-Kröll, Katharina & Wolfgang U. Dressler (2007). Diminutives and hypocoristics in Austrian German (AG). In I. Savickienė & W. U. Dressler (eds.) The acquisition of diminutives. A cross-linguistic perspective, 207–230. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Körtvélyessy, Lívia (2015). Evaluative morphology from a cross-linguistic perspective. Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Kramer, Ruth (2015). The Morphosyntax of Gender. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kučerová, Ivona (2018). φ-features at the Syntax-Semantics Interface: Evidence from Nominal Inflection. Linguistic Inquiry 49(4). 813–845.
Manova, Stela (2011). Understanding Morphological Rules: With Special Emphasis on Conversion and Subtraction in Bulgarian, Russian and Serbo-Croatian. Dordrecht: Springer.
Manova, Stela (2015). Affix order and the structure of the Slavic word. In S. Manova (ed.) Affix ordering across languages and frameworks, 205–230. New York: Oxford University Press.
Manova, Stela (to appear). Ordering restrictions between affixes. To appear in P. Ackema, S. Bendjaballah, E. Bonet & A. Fábregas (eds.) The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Morphology, also lingbuzz/006219
Manova, Stela & Georgia Knell (2021). Two-suffix combinations in native and non-native English: Novel evidence for morphomic structures. In S. Moradi, M. Haag, J. Rees-Miller & A. Petrovic (eds.) All Things Morphology: Its independence and its interfaces, 305–323. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Manova, Stela & Kimberley Winternitz (2011). Suffix order in double and multiple diminutives: with data from Polish and Bulgarian. Studies in Polish Linguistics 6: 115–138. URL: http://www.ejournals.eu/SPL/2011/SPL-vol-6-2011/art/1169/
Paster, Mary (2009). Explaining phonological conditions on affixation: Evidence from suppletive allomorphy and affix ordering. Word Structure 2(1): 18–37.
Rice, Keren (1989). A grammar of Slave. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Rice, Keren (2000). Morpheme Order and Semantic Scope. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Scalise, Sergio (1988). The notion of 'head' in morphology. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1. 229–245. Dordrecht: Foris.
Steriopolo, Olga (2009). Form and function of expressive morphology: A case study of Russian. Russian Language Journal 59: 149–194.
Steriopolo, Olga (2015). Syntactic variation in expressive size suffixes: a comparison of Russian, German, and Spanish. SKASE Journal of Theoretical Linguistics 12:2–21.
Steriopolo, Olga (2016). Expressives Across Languages: Form/Function Correlation. Annual Review of Linguistics 2:293–324.
Steriopolo, Olga6 (2017). Nominalizing evaluative suffixes in Russian: The interaction of declension class, gender, and animacy. Poljarnyj vestnik: Norwegian Journal of Slavic Studies 20: 18–44. Available at: http://septentrio.uit.no/index.php/vestnik/article/view/4141.
Stump, Gregory T. (1993). How Peculiar is Evaluative Morphology? Journal of Linguistics 29: 1–36.
Stump, Gregory T. (2001). Inflectional morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stump, Gregory T. (2016). Inflectional paradigms: content and form at the syntax-morphology interface. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Szymanek, Bogdan & Tetyana Derkach (2005). Constraints on the derivation of double diminutives in Polish and Ukrainian. Studies in Polish Linguistics 2: 93−112.
Wiltschko, Martina & Olga Steriopolo (2007). Parameters of variation in the syntax of diminutives. In M. Radišic (ed.) Proceedings of the 2007 Canadian Linguistics Association Annual Conference, http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/actes2007/Wiltschko_Steriopolo.pdf
Wunderlich, Dieter (1996). A minimalist model of inflectional morphology. In C. Wilder, H.-M. Gärtner & M. Bierwisch (eds.), The role of economy principles in linguistic theory, 267–298. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://listserv.linguistlist.org/pipermail/dgkl/attachments/20220126/b6fb26a9/attachment.htm>
More information about the DGKL
mailing list