"fieldwork"
Karl Teeter
kvt at husc.harvard.edu
Wed Feb 12 16:46:51 UTC 1997
We certainly seem to be getting into strange territory in this
discussion of fieldwork, ranging from flames (which have no
place on this
list, Mark) to all this about how you've got to actually live
in the
community to do it, which is nonsense. I started with actual
field work
in 1956, and it has been a major annual preoccupation of mine
from then
through 1989. I have lived in the communities, I have worked
in my
academic setting with native speakers, I have used native
speakers as
informants in field methods classes. All of this is serious
linguistic
fieldwork. Living in the community is desirable when
feasible, becoming
a quasi-native speaker when you have that ability (I do not,
except for
English) is also desirable. But happily, serious field
research is
possible without becoming a native speaker and without living
in the
community. Some go out into the "bush" and some do not, but
the former
does not guarantee their success nor the latter their
failure. Let us
see if we can't talk about field work without this
irrelevance.
As for books about field work, I know of two serious
attempts at
textbooks, one by Robbins Burling and one by William
Samarin. Both are
excellent attempts. I confess that I have thought of
attempting something
like this myself, but have found that hands-on
demonstration with a
native speaker is much more effective. For good books
aiming at doing
analysis through field work it we need to go back in
our history
to the 1940s and the old textbooks put out by
University of Michigan
Press: Kenneth L. Pike, PHONEMICS: A Technique for
Reducing Languages to
Writing, 1947, and Eugene A. Nida, MORPHOLOGY: The
Descriptive Analysis
of Words, 1949. The best book on field work, not a
textbook, is far and
away Bob Dixon's Searching for Aboriginal
Languages. But as with most
things, the best way to do it is to just go and do
it. In my own experience,
when I began with the last speaker of Wiyot, after all
kinds of university
courses, I was sent up to northernCalifornia with
notebooks and a tape recorder
and the instructions,"bring back a grammar,
dictionary, and texts". When, I got
there I found myself totally bemused by these
instructions. But I was
fortunate to find a woman passionate to avoid her
language's dying with
her, and we both worked hard, and now I have published
a grammar, texts,
and interim dictionary -- a comprehensive lexicon is
underway. Just be
cool and be serious, it can be done, and linguistic
fieldwork offers an
exciting route into the minds of others.
So okay, folks, more reminiscences and fewer
flames. Your friend,
kvt (=Karl V. Teeter, Professor of
Lingusitics, Emeritus, Harvard
University)
----
Endangered-Languages-L Forum:
endangered-languages-l at carmen.murdoch.edu.au
Web pages
http://carmen.murdoch.edu.au/lists/endangered-languages-l/
Subscribe/unsubscribe and other commands:
majordomo at carmen.murdoch.edu.au
----
=========================================================================
Date: Wed, 12 Feb 1997 13:27:31 -0800
From: Tom Payne <tpayne at OREGON.UOREGON.EDU>
Subject: Re: "fieldwork"
To:
endangered-languages-l at carmen.murdoch.edu.au
Organization: University of Oregon
MIME-version: 1.0
X-Mailer: Mozilla 3.0 (Win16; I)
Content-transfer-encoding: 7bit
References:
<Pine.OSF.3.95.970212112236.19464A-100000 at login1.fas.harvard.edu>
Sender:
owner-endangered-languages-l at carmen.murdoch.edu.au
Precedence: bulk
Reply-To:
endangered-languages-l at carmen.murdoch.edu.au
Content-Type: text/plain; charset=us-ascii
Content-Length: 3431
Karl Teeter recently posted a message in which he suggests
that contributors to this list refrain from "flames." He
then immediately caricatured a very reasonable perspective on
descriptive linguistics as "nonsense."
The reasonable perspective is that the best way to do
linguistic research is in the cultural setting in
which the language is spoken.
The caricature is that the only way to do linguistic research
is in the cultural setting in which the language is spoken.
I would agree with the reasonable perspective, and would
disagree with the caricature. But I see no reason to describe
the caricature as "nonsense." It may be wrong, but there
is some logic to it.
Lest some view Professor Teeter's flame as somehow
"proving" that the reasonable perspective is wrong (which
he didn't claim, but may be construed to have claimed), I
would like to mention again the many, many times
linguistic analyses based on decontextualized data have
been shown to be wrong or seriously incomplete when
exposed to the dynamic, uncontrolled, spontanaity of
language being used to accomplish social acts within the
cultural setting that the language is normally used.
Of course, the converse is also true. A linguistic
analysis based only on texts or conversational data may
conclude, for example, that a language has no second person declarative
forms. This is because it is extrememly uncommon for
individuals to inform others of what the others are doing:
"You are baking bread." In conversation, clauses with second person
subjects are overwhelmingly questions and commands. This
example illustrates that elicitation is absolutely
essential to fill out paradigms, and collect "inventories"
of various possible coding choice.
However, I would just disagree with the assertion that
linguistic research done outside of the cultural setting
of the speakers of the language is as sound as research
done in communities of speakers. I'm not sure if that is
the view Professor Teeter takes. Language is an expression
of culture, and learning a language, both for purposes of
analysis and speaking, is best done in a cultural setting
where that language has evolved.
Perhaps in a future post, if people are interested, I will
try to articulate a perspective on descriptive linguistics
as qualitative social science research. Linguistic
knowledge is "tacit" knowledge, and as such qualitative
methodologies, such as participant observation, are most
appropriate for illucidating it. Participant observation
may be discredited among those who view linguistics as
more of a "hard" science than a social science, but I
believe that there are certain questions about a language,
even the nuts and bolts of syntax and morphology, that
cannot be adequately understood using only quantitative
methods. But that is another topic, so I will leave it for
a future post, if there is any indication of interest.
_____________________________________________________________________
Thomas E. Payne
Department of Linguistics
University of Oregon
Eugene, OR 97403
USA
Voice: 541 342-6706
Fax: 541 346-3917
______________________________________________________________________
----
Endangered-Languages-L Forum: endangered-languages-l at carmen.murdoch.edu.au
Web pages http://carmen.murdoch.edu.au/lists/endangered-languages-l/
Subscribe/unsubscribe and other commands: majordomo at carmen.murdoch.edu.au
----
More information about the Endangered-languages-l
mailing list