Versatility?
Angus B. Grieve-Smith
grvsmth at panix.com
Sat Mar 19 17:15:30 UTC 2011
On 3/19/2011 12:23 PM, A. Katz wrote:
> In a way, that's a valid thing to say, since obviously the language
> works just fine without this kind of knowledge in speakers. But... it
> works differently from the way it would have worked without the
> massive borrowing that made the language's derivational system so
> irregular that speakers tend to disregard it when parsing words, even
> words like rooster whose component morphemes are both known to them.
I think speakers tend to parse sentences in frequent chunks and
often disregard their components. Everyone knows "cell" and "phone,"
but how many people think about cells when they say "cell phone"? Or
about Eskimos when eating Eskimo pie? Or facsimiles when sending
faxes? For me a drivers license is more useful in buying sudafed than
in driving.
Etymology can be interesting and fun; I regularly get a kick out of
realizing that the "efharesto" that I learned at the eye doctor's is the
same as the Eucharist, or that State Senator Dan Garodnick and transit
construction chief Michael Horodniceanu have the same last name. But
that's me, I'm interested in words. Etymology can teach us a lot when
we have time to study and contemplate it, but its day-to-day practical
application in understanding words is minor in any language.
One thing that I've learned from studying English's quirky
etymology is that it's often formed by analogy instead of
compositionality (think "intranet" which makes no sense from a
compositional standpoint, or "devil's food cake"). It might be that
languages that have more regular derivational morphology rely on
compositionality more, but it could also be that they work just as much
by analogy, but it's harder to tell the difference.
--
-Angus B. Grieve-Smith
grvsmth at panix.com
More information about the Funknet
mailing list