One internalist pespective on creoles and genetic classification
Michel DeGraff
degraff at MIT.EDU
Wed Mar 10 21:23:10 UTC 1999
----------------------------Original message----------------------------
RE message from bwald at humnet.ucla.edu on Tue, 2 Mar 1999 08:44:27 EST:
> ----------------------------Original message----------------------------
[...]
> Not so removed from this is the (former) issue of whether Haitian (Creole)
> is a Romance language. Until relatively recent times when pidgin-creole
> studies started to be taken seriously by historical linguists in general
> (or have they? AMR, to be sure, seems to take them seriously), lexicon
> alone (i.e, through regular sound correspondences) seems to have been
> criterial of inclusion in "language family" (for the "mainstream").
> Grammar did not count for much, just as it did not count for much in
> synchronic linguistic description.
[...]
> [...] such issues as Haitian brought up DEFINITIONAL problems of language
> family. How to classify when there is historical discontinuity in the
> grammar?
> (NB: I could say more about "political" motivations for different sides on
> whether or not Haitian should be [have been] included in Romance, but I am
> saying enough that can be misinterpreted without getting into that fruitful
> topic. Currently there is still disagreement about how to account for
> the grammar of Haitian and various other "creoles", e.g., whether it comes
> from an innate "bioprogram" or is relexified Fon (a Kwa Niger-Congo
> language), etc etc. There are also issues involving whether many so-called
> creoles actually descend from earlier pidgins, e.g., Berbice Dutch,
> sometimes called Berbice "Creole" Dutch, which seems to be a "mixture" of
> Dutch and Kalabari (the latter a variety of Ijoid, a branch of Niger-Congo
> that has contentious aspects for (sub)classification), or various
> non-European varieties of Portuguese that seem to have been "restructured",
> but not necessarily descended "whole cloth" from pidgins, etc etc. Does
> "(radical) restructuring" exclude them from their lexical source "family"
> affiliation? These seem to be matters of DEFINITION of "family", not
> whatever the historical "facts" may be)
[...]
Dear all,
Similar and related questions are being raised among creolists and
generativists with interests in language change and language acquisition.
As it turns out, various authors in a forthcoming MIT Press anthology
address these questions from an internalist, generativist perspective.
Such perspective, I do realize, is not shared by all members of this list,
but I find their observations quite relevant to the intringuing questions
raised by Benji Wald... Back to commercials: The book's title is LANGUAGE
CREATION AND LANGUAGE CHANGE: CREOLIZATION, DIACHRONY AND DEVELOPMENT
(DeGraff, ed., MIT Press, 1999). Publication information can be found at:
http://mitpress.mit.edu/book-home.tcl?isbn=0262041685
What I'll do here is just quote one relevant advert ... sorry, excerpt
... from pages 13-14 of my introductory chapter (from the raw,
pre-copyediting files). This chapter is (rather ambitiously) titled:
"Creolization, Language Change and Language Acquisition: A Prolegomenon".
Here it goes:
******************************************************************************
Pages 13-14
[...] Are the language-related cognitive processes responsible
for creolization also involved in instances of ordinary
acquisition and in (gradual) language change?
[...]
A positive answer to [that] question would connect creolization
phenomena to more general diachronic phenomena, that is, to the
better-understood instances of syntactic change occurring over
relatively long periods of time, as for example in the history of
English. Support for such a positive answer is given by
contributors' proposals whose aim is to account for
generalizations obtaining across cases of language change and
emergence; see Roberts's and Lightfoot's chapters for two such
proposals and Rizzi's and DeGraff's commentaries for further
discussion.
However, connecting creolization to language change [...] may at
first appear controversial. At the turn of the century,
Schuchardt (see Gilbert 1980b) used evidence from creole
languages, as many others have since, in attempts to refute the
Neogrammarian STAMMBAUMTHEORIE (``family-tree theory'') according
to which the parentage of each language goes through a SINGLE
ancestor; in this theory, languages reproduce asexually, so to
speak (see Thomason and Kaufman 1988 for a critique and
alternatives). With massive language contact in their histories,
creoles clearly belie the Stammbaumtheorie's ``one parent per
language'' assumption.
Some (e.g., Hall 1966, 117) have tried to maintain the
Neogrammarian assumption by unsuccessfully forcing creoles into
genetic affiliation with their superstrates. Others (e.g.,
Taylor 1956) have recognized two possible ways out: (a) either
creoles lie altogether outside Stammbaumtheorie (Taylor 1956,
407); cf. Thomason and Kaufman 1988, 9--12, 152, 165-166); or (b)
our theories must be revised and made more ``family-friendly'' to
allow for ``nongenetic'' relationships. An example of
possibility (b) is Taylor's (1956, 413) proposal that creoles are
``genetically `orphans' [with] two `foster-parents': one that
provides the basic morphological and/or syntactical pattern, and
another from which the fundamental vocabulary is taken''! In the
spirit of both (a) and (b), Thomason and Kaufman distinguish
between ``genetic'' and ``nongenetic'' paths of development, the
former arising via ``normal transmission'' and the latter via
``imperfect transmission'' as with abrupt creoles.
But is transmission ever ``perfect''? In the I-language
perspective adopted in this introduction, grammars are never
transmitted: they are always created anew from innate mental
resources (the language faculty plus acquisition and processing
mechanisms, say) coupled with the ambient (environment-specific)
PLD [Primary Linguistic Data] available to the learner (see
below).[Endnote 24 --- see below]
It is always the case that the PLD is both limited and
heterogeneous (in varying degrees), as a result of which the
final state of the language learner (i.e., the attained internal
grammar, which gives rise to unlimited productivity) is
inevitably underdetermined (see chapters 13--15 for further
discussion).
What Thomason and Kaufman (1988) call ``genetic'' versus
``nongenetic'' has no theoretical status in this framework: in
both cases (``genetic'' language change AND ``nongenetic''
creolization), the learner's normal task is to set parameters
using whatever PLD are available. In parameter-setting terms,
what Thomason and Kaufman's distinction might refer to with
respect to possibility (a) is the DEGREE of heterogeneity,
stability and/or complexity of the PLD in the genetic versus
nongenetic cases. In turn, the quality of the PLD is affected by
the many (socially determined) EXTERNAL factors that are at play
in all instances of language acquisition; two such factors, most
relevant to the creolization case, are (a) the varying fluencies
of the model speakers (i.e., those providing the PLD) in the
evolving common language, and (b) the diversity of the model
speakers' native tongues. The goal of this volume is to better
understand the structure of UG by studying the CONSTANT,
INTERNAL constraints on the outcomes of acquisition across
various sets of NONCONSTANT, EXTERNAL conditions. In my view,
such outcomes include those of both language change and
creolization. [...]
[...]
[p 40 Endnote 24:]
"In the words of Meillet (1929, 74),
[C]haque enfant doit acqu'erir par lui-m^eme la capacit'e de
comprendre le parler des gens de son groupe. ... La langue ne
lui est pas livre'e en bloc, tout d'une pie`ce. ... Pour chaque
individu, le langage est ainsi une recre'ation totale faite sous
l'influence du milieu qui l'entoure. Il ne saurait y avoir
discontinuite' plus absolue.
[Each child must on his own acquire the capacity to understand
the speech of people in his community. ... Language is not given
to him en bloc, all in one piece. ... Thus, for each individual,
language is a total re-creation, carried out under the influence
of the surrounding environment. There could not exist a more
absolute discontinuity. [my translation]]
******************************************************************************
Thank you,
-michel.
___________________________________________________________________________
MIT Linguistics & Philosophy, 77 Massachusetts Ave, Cambridge MA 02139-4307
http://web.mit.edu/linguistics/www/degraff.home.html
More information about the Histling
mailing list