Syntactic Bootstrapping - Summary
bartsch at zas.gwz-berlin.de
bartsch at zas.gwz-berlin.de
Thu Nov 1 08:37:21 UTC 2007
Dear all,
Some weeks ago I posted a query concerning a central issue in the Syntactic
Bootstrapping approach. Specifically, I was interested to know whether or
not SynBoot proponents claim that syntactic-semantic correlations underlying
SynBoot are innate.
First of all, many thanks for the replies of the following people:
Misha Becker
Ruth Berman
Gedeon Déak
Doug Harris
Evan Kidd
Lorraine McCune
Letitia Naigles, which sent me her paper Naigles & Swenson (2006)
Matthew Saxton
and a scholar preferring to remain anonym, who sent me passages from his
book which hes writing
The following summary is based on issues raised by the anonym scholars
message, including not only SynBoot proponents views. In the summary below,
responses by the other researchers listed above are integrated. This summary
includes only issues directly related to SynBoot. The discussion also raised
other relevant topics, such as innateness and the issue on relationships
between lexical/lexical-semantic and grammatical aspects in lg dev., above
all in the context of "emergence of grammar from the lexicon" (Bates &
Goodman, 1999), but these topics will be summarized in separate messages.
1. Introduction
- [
] the original proponents of the syntactic bootstrapping hypothesis
claim that [syntactic-semantic] links are not innately specified but are
acquired during the course of learning (Gleitman, 1990: 41).
- But different people make different assumptions (and those assumptions
aren't always easy to tie down, or test experimentally!)
- Evan: this one is hard to pin down, probably because there are many
different levels at which one could specify innate knowledge.
2. Innate knowledge
- The underlying knowledge on syntactic frames could be largely innate
(e.g., Pinker, 1989)
- Evan: my understanding is that Cindy Fisher and her colleagues argue for
an innate endowment that is based on structure-event role mapping, such that
children expect there to be, for instance, an agent and patient in
transitive sentence,and map these semantic roles onto the NPs in the
sentence. This, of course,could be traced to general-cognitive biases.
- Evan: Similarly, Lidz Gleitman, and Gleitman (2004) argued for innate
knowledge of the theta principle, to which Goldberg (2004) wrote a
response.
- Naigles & Swenson (2006: 221): Gleitman (1990; see also Fisher &
Gleitman, 2002; Fisher et al., 1991; Pinker, 1989) has proposed that
correspondences related to the theta criterion (Chomsky, 1981) are innately
given ***in contradiction with the quotation concerning Gleitman (1990)
above***, but it is perhaps the case that Naigles & Swenson and the anonym
researcher mean here different sets of syntactic-semantic correlations:
concerned with correspondences between semantic and syntactic roles (Naigles
& Swenson) or between syntactic-semantic correspondences in verbs, such as
transitivity-causality (the anonym researcher).
- Naigles & Swenson (2006): In Lidz et al.s (2003) view, "syntax/semantics
correspondences such as transitive/affected-object are universal and innate
(p. 228)
3. Role of Input and Domain-General Cognitive Abilities
3aThe underlying knowledge on syntactic frames could be constructed by
forming abstractions across instantiations of this pattern in the input
(e.g., Tomasello, 2003).
- this view is also found in Evans message: if there is no innate
knowledge then there is a lower bound on syntactic bootstrapping, such that
it only becomes useful once children have learned some construction-specific
frames
3b. Also, children might be guided by the number of arguments in the input
sentences (Fisher, 1996; 2002)
3c. Also, children might be guided by morphological cues in the input in
form of function words, such as 'and', 'with' or copulas ('are') to derive
an intransitive interpretation and, consequently, non-causative
interpretation, as shown most clearly in the study of Kidd et al (2001)
(***Id be most grateful for the full reference or even the full text of
this study by Kidd et al.***)
4. Naigles & Swensons (2006) conclusions:
- some such [syntactic] frames might carry general, possibly innate,
meanings that would be stable across languages. Further specificity for a
given verb could be gleaned from the accompanying scenes and/or the multiple
frames in which the verb was used. (p. 225)
- childrens abilities to learn some words without the syntactic
information, to compare the statistical and/or pragmatic patterns of use of
different types of words, and so to induce some syntax/semantics
correspondences by matching these words with specific morphosyntactic
patterns. (p. 226).
- The evidence concerning the development of syntactic bootstrapping
suggests that the use of syntax to learn novel words is fragile between 18
and 24 months of age and fully operational at 24 months, which suggests
that some syntax/semantics correspondences, if couched generally, could be
universal and so available before 18 months whereas other correspondences,
obviously language-specific, must be learned and so are not readily
available to 1-year-olds. (pp. 227-8)
- similar view by Misha Becker: The way I understand syntactic
bootstrapping is that there is innate knowledge of likelihoods/tendencies in
the mapping between sentence frames and meanings. [
] So it doesn't solve
the entire problem of lexical learning, it just provides some constraints in
the form of tendencies. My understanding is that these are innate, and then
language particular constraints or options (e.g. null arguments) come in by
way of learning.
5. Abstract syntactic categories in child language or not?
- Evan recommends the lecture of Fishers (2002) reply to Tomasello (2000).
Tomasello challenges the very SynBoot idea (syntactic structures facilitate
learning of words) in that he (as I think, correctly) assumes that
childrens representations underlying their early constructions are
lexically based and not abstract syntactic categories, such as verb or
transitivity. Fisher agree partially with that: Childrens abstract
syntactic categories develop gradually, However, incomplete syntactic
knowledge need not mean that abstract knowledge of sentence structure can
play no useful role in early language use and acquisition. (p. 272)
- Evan also recommends the lecture of Chang et al. (2006), a connectionist
study showing how abstract knowledge and concrete experience are balanced
in the development and use of syntax. (from abstract)
5. Syntactic and Distributional/Correlational Bootstrapping
- As Ruth Berman pointed out, SynBoot claims have been made mainly for verb
argument structure, not necessarily for other features of the grammar --
which is also an interesting question.
- Indeed, the more classical SynBoot strang is, as I understand it,
concerned with the learning of verb meanings (or rather: components of their
meanings, such as causality), i.e., with the induction problem of
inferring word meanings. This is in contrast with Distributional and
Semantic Bootstrapping, which are traditionally concerned with what Pinker
called the bootstrapping problem of categorization of words and syntactic
units.
- Interestingly enough, however, a number of recent studies by SynBoot
proponents (e.g., Booth & Waxman, 2003; Fisher et al., 2006; Bernal, 2006;
Name, 2007) or cited by them as providing support for SynBoot (e.g.,
Blenn et al., 2002; Höhle et al., 2006; Ketrez, 2003) constitute something
of a renaissance of the Distributional Bootrapping idea (Maratsos &
Chalkley, 1980; more recent accounts: Mintz, 2005; Freudenthal et al.,
2007): Their results suggests that distributional syntactic cues (e.g., word
order, determiner-noun cooccurrences, morphological markers) facilitate the
task of segmentation/categorization of lexical and syntactic units.
Finally, for the summary to be complete, the following comment by Doug
Harris, which, however, has nothing to do with bootstrapping operations of
(language) learning:
- Speaking of _bootstrapping_, I heard an interesting variation of that
(source) word the other day, I believe it was on NPR, while I was _so_
otherwise engaged I couldn't make a written note to myself on it: A man was
talking about, I believe, how his town would struggle together after some
disaster or other and "pull its bootstrings up" and work toward recovery, or
whatever. I'm sorry I can't be more precise on the source.
Again, many thanks and best regards
Susanna
References
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1999). On the emergence of grammar from the
lexicon. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The Emergence of language (pp. 2979).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bernal, S. (2006). De l'arbre (syntaxique) au fruit (du sens): Interactions
des acquisitions lexicale et syntaxique chez l'enfant de moins de 2 ans.
Doctoral dissertation. Paris: Université Paris VI.
Blenn, L., Seidl, A., & Höhle, B. (2002). Recognition of phrases in early
language acquisition: The role of morphological markers. In B. Skarabela, S.
Fish, & A. H.-J. Do (Eds.), BUCLD 26. Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston
University Conference on Language Development, November 2-4, 2001, in
Boston, MA, Vol. 1 (pp. 138149). Somerville Mass.: Cascadilla Press.
Booth, A. E., & Waxman, S. R. (2003). Mapping words to the world in infancy:
Infants' expectations for count nouns and adjectives. Journal of Cognition
and Development, 4, 357381.
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological
Review, 113(2), 234272.
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Amsterdam: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Fisher, C. (1996). Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of analogy in
children's interpretations of sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 31(1), 4181.
Fisher, C. (2002). The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language
acquisition: A reply to Tomasello (2000). Cognition, 82, 259278.
Fisher, C., & Gleitman, L. R. (2002). Language acquisition. In R. Gallistel,
& H. Pashler (Eds.), Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology /
ed.-in-chief Hal Pashler: Vol. Vol. 3. Learning, motivation, and emotion. 3.
ed (pp. 445496). New York, NY: Wiley.
Fisher, C., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1991). On the semantic content
of subcategorization frames. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 331392.
Fisher, C., Klingler, S. L., & Song, H.-j. (2006). What does syntax say
about space? 2-year-olds use sentence structure to learn new prepositions.
Cognition, 101(1), B19-B29.
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., & Gobet, F. R. (2007). Simulating the
noun-verb asymmetry in the productivity of children's speech. In R. L.
Lewis, A. Polk, & J. E. Laird (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th International
Conference on Cognitive Modelling (pp. 109114). Hove UK: Psychology Press.
Gleitman, L. R. (1990). The structural sources of verb meaning. Language
Acquisition, 1(1), 355.
Goldberg, A. E. (2004). But do we need Universal Grammar? Comment on Lidz et
al. (2003). Cognition, 94, 7784.
Höhle, B., Schmitz, M., Müller, A., & Weissenborn, J. (2006). The
lexicon/syntax interface in developing grammar: The role of function words
in the acquisition of content words. ISIS 2006. XVth Biennal International
Society on Infant Studies Conference, Kyoto, Japan, Jun 19-23, 2006.
Ketrez, N. (2003). Is it possible to bootstrap any lexical category
information from word order in a flexible-word-order language? BOOT-LA:
Bootstrapping in Language Acquisition: Psychological, Linguistic, and
Computational Aspects, Bloomington, Apr 21-23, 2003.
Kidd et al (2001) ???
Lidz, J. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (2003). Understanding how input
matters: Verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar. Cognition,
87(3), 151178.
Lidz, J. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (2004). Kidz in the 'hood:
Syntactic bootstrapping and the mental lexicon. In D. Geoffrey Hall, & S. R.
Waxman (Eds.), From many strands. Weaving a lexicon (pp. 603636).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Maratsos, M., & Chalkley, M. (1980). The internal language of children's
syntax: The ontogenesis and representation of syntactic categories. In K. E.
Nelson (Ed.), Children's language. Vol. 2. New York NY: Gardner.
Mintz, T. (2005). Categorizing words from distributional information in the
input. Paper presented at the symposium "The Interaction of Input and
Learning Mechanisms in Language Acquisition: Four Case Studies", Xth
International Congress for the Study of Child Language (X IASCL Congress),
Berlin, July 25-29, 2005.
Naigles, L., & Swensen, L. D. (2006). Syntactic supports for word learning.
In E. Hoff, & M. Shatz (Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Language Development.
London: Blackwell Publishing.
Name, M. Cristina. (2007). Bootstrapping sintático: O papel da ordem
estrutural na aquisição de nomes e adjetivos [Syntactic bootstrapping: The
role of structural order in the acquisition of nouns and adjectives]. Letras
de Hoje, 42(1), 5363.
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and Cognition: The acquisition of argument
structure. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Tomasello, M. (2000). Do children have adult syntactic competence?
Cognition, 74, 209253.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of
language acquisition. Cambridge Mass.: Harvard Univ. Press.
*****************************************************************
Susanna Bartsch
https://www.zas.gwz-berlin.de/mitarb/homepage/bartsch/
bartsch at zas.gwz-berlin.de
Zentrum fuer Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft, Typologie
und Universalienforschung (ZAS)
Centre for General Linguistics, Typology, and Universals Research
Schuetzenstr. 18
10117 Berlin
Germany
Tel. +49 (0)30 20192562
Fax +49 (0)30 20192402
*****************************************************************
--~--~---------~--~----~------------~-------~--~----~
You received this message because you are subscribed to the Google Groups "Info-CHILDES" group.
To post to this group, send email to info-childes at googlegroups.com
To unsubscribe from this group, send email to info-childes-unsubscribe at googlegroups.com
For more options, visit this group at http://groups.google.com/group/info-childes?hl=en
-~----------~----~----~----~------~----~------~--~---
More information about the Info-childes
mailing list