Dunn et al. on word order typology in "Nature"
Bill Croft
wcroft at UNM.EDU
Fri Apr 15 15:59:44 UTC 2011
We should not lose sight of what is good in Dunn et al.'s Nature
article. Many typologists, starting with Greenberg, have argued that
synchronic language universals are really just manifestations of
diachronic universals. The state-process model used by Dunn et al.
(see p. 5 of their supplementary materials) has been used by
Greenberg (1978) and Maslova (2000). Dunn et al.'s method (and also
Maslova's) allows us to take a major problem with sampling -
historical dependence - and exploit it to uncover valid language
universals. Using quantitative and statistical techniques will allow
us to make more precise generalizations and assign a degree of
goodness of fit to our theoretical models of typological universals.
Nevertheless, like others here, I am unconvinced of the results due
to problems with the way they apply the method. Dunn et al.'s
analysis (pp. 5-6 of the supplementary materials) treats two models
as mutually exclusive: an "independent" model, in which a word order
switches independently of other orders, possibly with a weighted
preference for one order; and a "dependent" model, in which two word
orders are linked and change together. These competing models are
tested against each phylogeny (family tree), with one or the other
winning out (*very* crudely, if branches with linked changes
outnumber branches with single order changes, then the test takes the
word-order correlation as justified for the tree). But this does not
test the model that most typologists assume; at best it tests the
oversimplified model of Theo Vennemann and Winfrid Lehmann from the
1970s, which reduced the diversity of word order in the world's
languages to just two types, VO and OV. (This is also basically the
generative head-ordering parameter.) But from Greenberg's original
paper (Greenberg 1966) onwards, most typologists have adopted a model
in which both single-order preferences (Greenberg's 'dominance') and
linkages (Greenberg's 'harmony') compete with each other and jointly
determine patterns of word order variation, as Matthew noted in his
last post. This can be demonstrated statistically: Justeson and
Stephens (1990) did a log-linear analysis on a large synchronic
language sample and showed that the best-fit model included both
dominance and harmony factors. I presume there is a dynamic
equivalent of log-linear analysis that could be used to test the
model that most typologists accept.
Regarding the issue of lineage-specific vs. universal patterns: Keith
Poole and I had to deal with a similar question in using
multidimensional scaling to find universals of grammatical categories
(Croft and Poole 2008). There, the contrast was between
language-specific MDS models (as used, for example, by Barbara Malt
and her colleagues) and crosslinguistic MDS models. We argued that if
the regularities were culture-specific, then mixing in languages with
culturally-specific category structures would reduce the goodness of
fit of the MDS model; but if the regularities were crosslinguistic
(i.e. universal), then mixing together languages would improve the
goodness of fit of the model. (We found the latter.) The same
presumably would apply for word order patterns, mutatis mutandis. But
here the problem with the Dunn et al. result is that they test only
four lineages, representing only 7.5% of language genera (low-level
language families) in the world; the other 92.5% of language genera
occur in other lineages. Also, for many of the pairwise correlations
they test, including the two illustrated in their article, Bantu is
too shallow a family to exhibit any variation; and for the two
illustrated in their article, even in the other families very few
branches undergo a word order shift. So the empirical sample, though
well distributed geographically, is very small and has few
independent changes to evaluate. And here we hit a problem that all
researchers on language universals hit: our uncertainty about
language phylogeny in most parts of the world, especially for deeper
families, but even for subgrouping in accepted families. In this
latter area, quantitative methods are also being applied; but that is
another story.
I think typologists should welcome this effort to marry phylogeny and
typology, even if we remain unconvinced of the particular result in
this paper.
Bill
Croft, William and Keith T. Poole. 2008. Inferring universals from
grammatical variation: multidimensional scaling for typological
analysis. Theoretical Linguistics 34.1-37.
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1966. Some universals of grammar with particular
reference to the order of meaningful elements. Universals of Grammar,
ed. Joseph H. Greenberg, 2nd edition, 73-113. Cambridge, Mass: MIT
Press
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1978. Diachrony, synchrony and language
universals. Universals of Human Language, Vol. 1: Method and Theory,
ed. Joseph H. Greenberg, Charles A. Ferguson and Edith A. Moravcsik,
61-92. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Maslova, Elena. 2000. A dynamic approach to the verification of
distributional universals. Linguistic Typology 4.307-33.
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://listserv.linguistlist.org/pipermail/lingtyp/attachments/20110415/49d9c7bc/attachment.htm>
More information about the Lingtyp
mailing list