[Lingtyp] Structural congruence

Matthew Dryer dryer at buffalo.edu
Wed Jan 20 03:35:12 UTC 2016


Randy,

I am more than willing to adopt the rhetorical stance that if I think I 
disagree with you, the default is that it is that I am misunderstanding 
what you are saying. As a rhetorical move, I will argue why I think you 
are mistaken when you say something, not to prove you are wrong, but to 
provide you with the opportunity to clarify what you mean.

However, I suspect that we are in broad agreement about what is the 
right thing to do at the level of describing individual languages. To 
me, describing or analysing a particular language is a completely 
different enterprise from classifying the language typologically, except 
that the former is a necessary prerequisite for the latter. The former 
should be describing the language entirely in its own terms, rather than 
trying to fit it into some typology. I am also strongly opposed to 
questionnaire-based typology, since if one is classifying phenomenon X 
in a set of languages, one needs to see how phenomenon X in a given 
language fits into the overall grammatical system of the language. I 
suspect we are broad agreement on those issues.

Second, since the number of possible types is presumably infinite, 
typology necessarily lumps together languages which are some level are 
clearly different. Even if I lump together types of languages which 
differ in some crucial way in their grammatical system, I feel obligated 
to justify grouping them together. That is the basis for my grouping 
together languages in which there is a grammatical rule that defines the 
language as what I would call SVO and languages where there are no 
syntactic rules governing clause word order, but where the pragmatic 
factors governing word order and the frequency in discourse in which 
those different pragmatic factors arise result in more frequent SVO 
order at the level of frequency in usage.

Third, it is important to distinguish terminological “disagreements” 
(which are not really disagreements) from substantive disagreements. 
Part of the difference in our discussion is based on our using the terms 
“subject” and “object” differently. I use it these terms as they are 
used in the vast majority of grammatical descriptions produced in the 
past 100 years, one which is roughly equivalent to the A and P in 
“basic” constructions (like active as opposed to passive). I recognize, 
however, that there are many linguists who use these terms differently.

When you say “Simply lumping them together under a single rubric does 
nothing but categorise them, and doesn’t explain anything”, the first 
part is not really true, on my view, if the basis for lumping them 
together is that they behave the same with respect to some explanatory 
enterprise. I.e. lumping them together is a necessary step towards 
explanation. You are rightthat lumping them together explains nothing. 
But NOT lumping together in the case we are discussing only leads, in my 
view, to mistaken explanations, since if we restricted our attention to 
languages in which the VO order reflected a syntactic rule in the 
language, we might be lead to explanations in terms of grammatical rules 
of the sort that are popular in some versions of generative grammar. The 
fact that languages which are VO in my terms only due to frequency in 
usage tend to exhibit the word order characteristics associated with VO 
word order means that we can rule out those lines of explanation that 
depend crucially on syntactic rules.

Matthew

On 1/19/16 9:44 AM, Randy John LaPolla (Prof) wrote:
> Sorry, Matthew, no. I am arguing it is an empirical question, and am 
> against the a priori assumption and imposition of categories on 
> languages without any basis. This is what I have been fighting against 
> for almost 30 years.
>
> We seem to be talking past each other, as we each have our own way of 
> understanding things, and assume the others are saying something they 
> aren’t. It seems impossible to post to this list without being 
> misunderstood. This is nice evidence for the theory of communication 
> I’ve been flogging for 20 years, but it is very frustrating.
>
> I was talking about the inductive analysis of individual languages. 
> You are talking about cross-linguistic characterisations. I also argue 
> there are no universal categories, and I think we need to understand 
> each language on its own terms, and in a description of an individual 
> language, which is what I am talking about, you need to give the facts 
> of that language, not a cross-linguistic category that also happens to 
> have the same name as a category that many people ascribe to 
> individual languages, such that people reading that description will 
> assume the language has that category.
>
> Also, what does the category “subject” mean to you such that it would 
> be cross-linguistically useful, to the point of even saying languages 
> that don’t have such a category are subject-verb-object languages?
>
> In terms of the correlations you talk about among languages that 
> manifest what is (from my view problematically) subsumed under the VO 
> or SVO rubric, my view is that we should look for the reasons why, in 
> terms of information structure, structural pivots, historical 
> development, or whatever, the languages manifest the particular 
> patterns they do. Simply lumping them together under a single rubric 
> does nothing but categorise them, and doesn’t explain anything.
>
> Randy
>
>
>> On 19 Jan 2016, at 9:01 pm, Matthew Dryer <dryer at buffalo.edu 
>> <mailto:dryer at buffalo.edu>> wrote:
>>
>> My point is actually independent of the question of whether there are 
>> crosslinguistic categories.Even if there are/were crosslinguistic 
>> categories, it doesn’t follow that typological classification is 
>> based on those categories. My statement that “classifying languages 
>> typologically does not entail that the terms employed in the 
>> typological classification correspond to categories in the language” 
>> is consistent with a position that classifying languages 
>> typologically sometimes classifies them on the basis of 
>> crosslinguistic categories and sometimes on the basis of 
>> semantically-defined notions or other notions independent of 
>> crosslinguistic categories. Randy’s statement that classifying a 
>> language as SVO implies that the language has categories of subject 
>> and object seems to imply that typological classification MUST be 
>> based on categories that exist within the individual languages.
>>
>> But there is a good argument that in the case in question, any 
>> typological classification that was based on categories that exist in 
>> individual languages and on languages in which word order codes 
>> subject and object would be inadequate. As I argued in Dryer (1989), 
>> languages in which word order does not code grammatical relations and 
>> in which the word order is not based on grammatical relations but in 
>> which VO word order is more common tend to have word order properties 
>> associated with VO word order, like prepositions, while analogous 
>> languages in which OV word order is more common tend to have word 
>> order properties associated with OV word order, like postpositions. 
>> What this means is that the GRAMMARS of what I classify as VO 
>> languages have nothing in common. It is only the languages that have 
>> something in common at the level of usage. Hence any notion of SVO 
>> language restricted to languages in which there are subject and 
>> object categories and in which word order is determined by 
>> grammatical relations will necessarily fail as the basis of word 
>> order correlations.
>>
>> The problem with Randy’s position (and perhaps Jan’s) is that he is 
>> making an a priori assumption on what is actually an empirical question.
>>
>> Matthew
>>
>> On 1/18/16 11:12 PM, Martin Haspelmath wrote:
>>> Unfortunately, to many people (not only generativists) it isn't 
>>> obvious at all that "classifying languages typologically does not 
>>> entail that the terms employed in the typological classification 
>>> correspond to categories in the language" (in other words, that 
>>> comparative concepts are distinct from descriptive categories).
>>>
>>> It seems that the default assumption of many people when they hear a 
>>> term like "dative" or "clitic" is that they are concepts like 
>>> "copper" or "red fox", i.e. natural kinds that exist independently 
>>> of individual language systems, just as red foxes can be recognized 
>>> independently of their habitats, and copper can even be recognized 
>>> independently of the planet on which is occurs. This is false, but 
>>> it hasn't been very widely recognized.
>>>
>>> In the 1980s, typologists discovered the important differences 
>>> between agents, topics, and syntactic pivots (as noted by Randy), 
>>> but such more fine-grained categories are still not sufficient for 
>>> describing any language. Agents can be different across languages, 
>>> topics can be different, and syntactic pivots can be different. 
>>> Thus, even "agent", "topic" and "pivot" can only be used as 
>>> comparative concepts, not as universally applicable descriptive 
>>> categories that would somehow have the same meaning in different 
>>> languages.
>>>
>>> Thus, it is not just confusing terminology (like Y.R. Chao's 
>>> "subject"), but also the presupposition that categories can be 
>>> carried over from one language to another that has confused linguists.
>>>
>>> Martin
>>>
>>> On 19.01.16 07:52, Matthew Dryer wrote:
>>>> Randy says that calling Chinese SVO implies that Chinese has such 
>>>> categories. I am surprised that he would say that. I would have 
>>>> thought it was obvious that classifying languages typologically 
>>>> does not entail that the terms employed in the typological 
>>>> classification correspond to categories in the language. Nor does 
>>>> it mean that these categories determine or are determined by word 
>>>> order. I have certainly made that clear in my work that classifying 
>>>> a language as SVO makes no claim about the categories in the 
>>>> language, nor that these categories determine word order even if 
>>>> the language has such categories.
>>>>
>>>> Matthew
>>>>
>>>> On 1/18/16 7:42 PM, Randy John LaPolla (Prof) wrote:
>>>>> Dan’s point is very important. For example, most people describing 
>>>>> languages do not know how to distinguish agents, topics, and 
>>>>> syntactic pivots (“subject”), and just call anything that occurs 
>>>>> initially as “subject”. Sometimes even when the linguist is clear 
>>>>> on the difference, they still use the word “subject”. E.g. Y. R. 
>>>>> Chao, in his grammar of spoken Chinese, clearly stated there is 
>>>>> nothing like what is referred to as “subject” in English, as all 
>>>>> clauses are simply topic-comment, but he still used the term 
>>>>> “subject” for what he said was purely a topic. This has confused 
>>>>> generations of linguists, and they call Chinese SVO, which not 
>>>>> only implies that Chinese has such categories, but also that these 
>>>>> categories either determine or are determined by word order. See 
>>>>> the following paper arguing against the use of such shortcuts, and 
>>>>> arguing for more careful determination of the factors determining 
>>>>> word order in a language:
>>>>>
>>>>> LaPolla, Randy J. & Dory Poa. 2006. On describing word order. 
>>>>> /Catching Language: The Standing Challenge of Grammar Writing, 
>>>>> /ed. by Felix Ameka, Alan Dench, & Nicholas Evans, 269-295. 
>>>>> Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
>>>>>
>>>>> <http://randylapolla.net/papers/LaPolla_and_Poa_2006_On_Describing_Word_Order.pdf>http://randylapolla.net/papers/LaPolla_and_Poa_2006_On_Describing_Word_Order.pdf
>>>>>
>>>>>
>>>>> Randy
>>>>> -----
>>>>> *Prof. Randy J. LaPolla, PhD FAHA* (羅仁 地)| Division of 
>>>>> Linguistics and Multilingual Studies | Nanyang Technological 
>>>>> University
>>>>> HSS-03-45, 14 Nanyang Drive, Singapore 637332 | Tel: (65) 
>>>>> 6592-1825 GMT+8h | Fax: (65) 6795-6525 | 
>>>>> <http://randylapolla.net/>http://randylapolla.net/
>>>>>
>>>>>
>>>>>
>>>>>> On 19 Jan 2016, at 10:21 am, Everett, Daniel 
>>>>>> <DEVERETT at bentley.edu <mailto:DEVERETT at bentley.edu>> wrote:
>>>>>>
>>>>>> One of the biggest problems in this regard that I have noticed is 
>>>>>> in grammars of individual languages. Fieldworkers sometimes 
>>>>>> confuse semantic and formal categories in the grammars, 
>>>>>> classifying as a syntactic structure a semantic category. If 
>>>>>> typologists are not careful writers/readers of grammars they may 
>>>>>> bring such confusions into their typological studies. Sounds 
>>>>>> obvious. But not always so.
>>>>>>
>>>>>> Dan
>>>>>>
>>>>>> Sent from my iPhone
>>>>>>
>>>>>> On Jan 18, 2016, at 21:11, Matthew Dryer <dryer at buffalo.edu> wrote:
>>>>>>
>>>>>>> I agree entirely with Jan on the need to distinguish semantic 
>>>>>>> categories and formal categories. In fact, in a paper of mine 
>>>>>>> that is I have nearly completed revising, I have an entire 
>>>>>>> section arguing that generative approaches fail to note the fact 
>>>>>>> that a given semantic category often has many different formal 
>>>>>>> expressions over different languages and that this is 
>>>>>>> problematic for implicit assumptions that equate semantic 
>>>>>>> categories with formal categories.
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>> But Jan seems to think that this presents some sort of problem 
>>>>>>> for the work I have done in word order typology.He says “When 
>>>>>>> these authors subsequently formulate rules and principles on the 
>>>>>>> basis of the data they collected, the semantic category labels 
>>>>>>> (Adjective, Genitive, Relative Clause, but also e.g. 
>>>>>>> Demonstrative and Numeral) appear to stand for _formal_ 
>>>>>>> categories, i.e. categories whose members are defined on the 
>>>>>>> basis of structural or morphosyntactic criteria”. But this is 
>>>>>>> false. They stand for semantic categories.
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>> Jan seems to think that it is somehow a problem that a given 
>>>>>>> semantic category may have many different formal realizations 
>>>>>>> across different languages. However, neither in his email nor in 
>>>>>>> his 2009 paper in LT does he explain why he sees this as a problem.
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>> There is, I admit, a /potential/ problem.Namely, it might be the 
>>>>>>> case that for the purposes of word order correlations, the 
>>>>>>> syntactic realization of a semantic category makes a major 
>>>>>>> difference and that lumping the different syntactic realizations 
>>>>>>> together is obscuring these differences. That is why I have 
>>>>>>> spent considerable time over the years collecting data, not only 
>>>>>>> on word order in particular languages, but also on the syntactic 
>>>>>>> realization in these languages, precisely to examine empirically 
>>>>>>> whether the syntactic realization makes a difference. The result 
>>>>>>> is that while the syntactic realization sometimes makes a small 
>>>>>>> difference, it is overall irrelevant: by and large, 
>>>>>>> generalizations over semantic categories apply the same, 
>>>>>>> regardless of the syntactic realization.
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>> Matthew
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>> On 1/18/16 4:41 AM, Jan Rijkhoff wrote:
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> I think the last word has not been said about Greenbergian word 
>>>>>>>> order correlations, mainly because semantic categories and 
>>>>>>>> formal categories have not always been clearly distinguished in 
>>>>>>>> post-Greenberg (1963) word order studies (Rijkhoff 2009a).* For 
>>>>>>>> example, both Hawkins (1983: 12) and Dryer (1992: 120) claimed 
>>>>>>>> that they followed Greenberg (1963: 74) in ‘basically applying 
>>>>>>>> semantic criteria’ to identify members of the same category 
>>>>>>>> across languages, but in practice these semantically defined 
>>>>>>>> forms and constructions are treated as formal entities.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> If Hawkins and Dryer applied semantic criteria in their 
>>>>>>>> cross-linguistic studies, this implies, for example, that their 
>>>>>>>> semantic category Adjective must also have included verbal and 
>>>>>>>> nominal expressions of adjectival notions (such as relative 
>>>>>>>> clauses and genitives), which are typically used in languages 
>>>>>>>> that lack a dedicated class of adjectives:
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> _Kiribati _(Ross 1998: 90)
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> (1)///teueeaeetikiraoi/(relative clause)
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> artflower rel 3sg.s be.pretty
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> ‘a pretty flower’ (lit. ‘a flower that pretties’)
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> _Makwe_(Devos 2008: 136)
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> (2)/muú-nuw-á=ki-búúli/(genitive)
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> nc1-person pp1-gen=nc7-silence
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> ‘a silent person’ (lit. ‘person of silence’)
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Relative Clause and Genitive are, however, also semantic 
>>>>>>>> categories in their own right in word order studies by Dryer 
>>>>>>>> and Hawkins.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> When these authors subsequently formulate rules and principles 
>>>>>>>> on the basis of the data they collected, the semantic category 
>>>>>>>> labels (Adjective, Genitive, Relative Clause, but also e.g. 
>>>>>>>> Demonstrative and Numeral) appear to stand for _formal_ 
>>>>>>>> categories, i.e. categories whose members are defined on the 
>>>>>>>> basis of structural or morphosyntactic criteria. This apparent 
>>>>>>>> change of category is not explained, but can be seen in the 
>>>>>>>> case of the ‘Heaviness Serialization Principle’ (Hawkins 1983: 
>>>>>>>> 90-91) and the ‘Branching Direction Theory’ (Dryer 1992).
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Hawkins defined ‘heaviness’ in terms of such non-semantic 
>>>>>>>> criteria as (a) length and quantity of morphemes, (b) quantity 
>>>>>>>> of words, (c) syntactic depth of branching nodes, and (d) 
>>>>>>>> inclusion of dominated constituents.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> (3)// /Heaviness Serialization Principle/: Rel≥_R Gen≥_R A≥_R 
>>>>>>>> Dem/Num
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Thus a member of the (semantic? formal?) category Relative 
>>>>>>>> Clause is ‘heavier’ than a member of the (semantic? formal?) 
>>>>>>>> category Adjective. But Hawkins’s semantic category Adjective 
>>>>>>>> must also have included members of the ‘heavy’ formal 
>>>>>>>> categories Genitive and Relative Clause (see (1) and (2) 
>>>>>>>> above). It is not clear whether the original members of the 
>>>>>>>> single semantic category Adjective were later ‘re-categorized’ 
>>>>>>>> and distributed over the formal categories Adjective, Genitive 
>>>>>>>> and Relative Clause in the /Heaviness Serialization Principle/.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Dryer’s ‘Branching Direction Theory’ refers to a structural 
>>>>>>>> feature of the internal syntactic organization of a 
>>>>>>>> constituent. According to the ‘Branching Direction Theory’, 
>>>>>>>> relative clauses and genitives are phrases, i.e. members of a 
>>>>>>>> branching category, whose position relative to the noun 
>>>>>>>> correlates with the relative order of Verb and Object, whereas 
>>>>>>>> adjectives are non-branching elements, whose position relative 
>>>>>>>> to the noun does not correlate with OV or VO order (Dryer 1992: 
>>>>>>>> 107-8, 110-1). In this case, too, one may assume that the 
>>>>>>>> semantic category Adjective also included members of the formal 
>>>>>>>> categories Genitive and Relative Clause (see examples above). 
>>>>>>>> Again we do not know what happened to the branching/phrasal 
>>>>>>>> members of the erstwhile(?) semantic category Adjective 
>>>>>>>> (relative clauses, genitives) when this category was turned 
>>>>>>>> into the formal (non-branching) category Adjective that is part 
>>>>>>>> of the ‘Branching Direction Theory’.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> So as to avoid categorial confusion in cross-linguistic 
>>>>>>>> research (and so as to make it possible to produce more 
>>>>>>>> reliable results), it is necessary to keep formal and semantic 
>>>>>>>> categories apart, as members of these two categories have their 
>>>>>>>> own ordering rules or preferences. I also think it is an 
>>>>>>>> illusion to think we can give a satisfactory account of the 
>>>>>>>> grammatical behaviour of linguistic units -including word 
>>>>>>>> order- without taking into consideration functional 
>>>>>>>> (interpersonal) categories or ‘discourse units’ (Rijkhoff 
>>>>>>>> 2009b, 2015).
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> * Greenberg (1963: 88) made it clear that he sometimes used 
>>>>>>>> formal criteria to remove certain members of a semantic 
>>>>>>>> category before he formulated a universal, as in the case of 
>>>>>>>> his Universal 22.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> *References*
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Devos, M. 2008. /A Grammar of Makwe/. München: Lincom Europa.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Dryer, M. S., 1992. The Greenbergian word order correlations. 
>>>>>>>> /Language/ 68-1, 81-138.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Greenberg, J. H. 1963. Some universals of grammar with 
>>>>>>>> particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. 
>>>>>>>> H. Greenberg (ed.), /Universals of Language/, 73-113. Cambridge 
>>>>>>>> MA: MIT.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Hawkins, J. A., 1983. /Word Order Universals: Quantitative 
>>>>>>>> analyses of linguistic structure/. New York: Academic Press.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Rijkhoff, J. 2009a. On the (un)suitability of semantic 
>>>>>>>> categories. /Linguistic Typology/ 13-1, 95‑104.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Rijkhoff, Jan. 2009b. On the co-variation between form and 
>>>>>>>> function of adnominal possessive modifiers in Dutch and 
>>>>>>>> English. In William B. McGregor (ed.), /The Expression of 
>>>>>>>> Possession/ (The Expression of Cognitive Categories [ECC] 
>>>>>>>> 2),51‑106. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Rijkhoff, J. 2015. Word order. In James D. Wright 
>>>>>>>> (editor-in-chief), /International Encyclopedia of the Social & 
>>>>>>>> Behavioral Sciences (Second Edition)/, Vol. 25, 644–656. 
>>>>>>>> Oxford: Elsevier.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Ross, M. 1998. Proto-Oceanic adjectival categories and their 
>>>>>>>> morphosyntax. /Oceanic Linguistics/ 37-1, 85-119.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Jan Rijkhoff
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> ------------------------------------------------------------------------
>>>>>>>> *From:* Lingtyp [lingtyp-bounces at listserv.linguistlist.org] on 
>>>>>>>> behalf of Alan Rumsey [Alan.Rumsey at anu.edu.au]
>>>>>>>> *Sent:* Monday, January 18, 2016 12:23 PM
>>>>>>>> *To:* lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
>>>>>>>> *Subject:* Re: [Lingtyp] Structural congruence as a dimension 
>>>>>>>> of language complexity/simplicity
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Many thanks to all of you who responded to my posting on this 
>>>>>>>> topic, both online and off. All the readings you have pointed 
>>>>>>>> me to have indeed been highly relevant and very useful, 
>>>>>>>> including an excellent recent publication by Jennifer 
>>>>>>>> Culbertson that she pointed me to in her offline response, at 
>>>>>>>> http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01964/abstract
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Thanks especially to Matthew Dryer for pointing out that the 
>>>>>>>> Greenbergian ‘universal’ I had used as an example – the 
>>>>>>>> putative association between VSO and noun-adjective order — had 
>>>>>>>> been falsified by his much more thorough 1992 study “The 
>>>>>>>> Greenbergian Word Order Correlations”.  My reading of that 
>>>>>>>> article and further correspondence with him has confirmed that, 
>>>>>>>> by contrast, Greenberg’s universals no 3 and 4 were solidly 
>>>>>>>> confirmed by his study, namely that SOV languages are far more 
>>>>>>>> likely to have postpositions than prepositions and that the 
>>>>>>>> reverse is true for VSO  languages.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Drawing on all your suggestions, Francesca and I have now 
>>>>>>>> finished a draft of the paper referred to in my posting, called 
>>>>>>>> 'Structural Congruence as a Dimension of Language Complexity: 
>>>>>>>> An Example from Ku Waru Child Language’.**If any of you would 
>>>>>>>> like to read it please let me know and I’ll send it to you.
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> Alan
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>>
>>>>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>>>>> Lingtyp mailing list
>>>>>>>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
>>>>>>>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>>>>>>
>>>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>>>> Lingtyp mailing list
>>>>>>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org 
>>>>>>> <mailto:Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org>
>>>>>>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>>> Lingtyp mailing list
>>>>>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org 
>>>>>> <mailto:Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org>
>>>>>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>>>>
>>>>> ------------------------------------------------------------------------
>>>>> CONFIDENTIALITY: This email is intended solely for the person(s) 
>>>>> named and may be confidential and/or privileged. If you are not 
>>>>> the intended recipient, please delete it, notify us and do not 
>>>>> copy, use, or disclose its contents.
>>>>> Towards a sustainable earth: Print only when necessary. Thank you.
>>>>>
>>>>>
>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>> Lingtyp mailing list
>>>>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
>>>>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>>>
>>>>
>>>>
>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>> Lingtyp mailing list
>>>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
>>>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>>
>>> -- 
>>> Martin Haspelmath (haspelmath at shh.mpg.de)
>>> Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History
>>> Kahlaische Strasse 10	
>>> D-07745 Jena
>>> &
>>> Leipzig University
>>> Beethovenstrasse 15
>>> D-04107 Leipzig
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>> _______________________________________________
>>> Lingtyp mailing list
>>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
>>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>
>> _______________________________________________
>> Lingtyp mailing list
>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org 
>> <mailto:Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org>
>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>

-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://listserv.linguistlist.org/pipermail/lingtyp/attachments/20160119/867e3820/attachment.htm>


More information about the Lingtyp mailing list