16.599, Review: Syntax: Lechner (2004)
LINGUIST List
linguist at linguistlist.org
Wed Mar 2 05:43:13 UTC 2005
LINGUIST List: Vol-16-599. Wed Mar 02 2005. ISSN: 1068 - 4875.
Subject: 16.599, Review: Syntax: Lechner (2004)
Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Wayne State U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>
Reviews (reviews at linguistlist.org)
Sheila Collberg, U of Arizona
Terry Langendoen, U of Arizona
Homepage: http://linguistlist.org/
The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University, Wayne
State University, and donations from subscribers and publishers.
Editor for this issue: Naomi Ogasawara <naomi at linguistlist.org>
================================================================
What follows is a review or discussion note contributed to our
Book Discussion Forum. We expect discussions to be informal and
interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially
invited to join in. If you are interested in leading a book
discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available
for review." Then contact Sheila Collberg at collberg at linguistlist.org.
===========================Directory==============================
1)
Date: 01-Mar-2005
From: Lucia Grimaldi < grimaldi at lingrom.fu-berlin.de >
Subject: Ellipsis in Comparatives
-------------------------Message 1 ----------------------------------
Date: Wed, 02 Mar 2005 00:40:56
From: Lucia Grimaldi < grimaldi at lingrom.fu-berlin.de >
Subject: Ellipsis in Comparatives
Editor's note: This issue contains non-ISO-8859-1 characters.
To view the correct characters, go to http://linguistlist.org/issues/16/16-599.html.
AUTHOR: Lechner, Winfried
TITLE: Ellipsis in Comparatives
SERIES: Studies in Generative Grammar 72
PUBLISHER: Mouton de Gruyter
YEAR: 2004
Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/15/15-2946.html
Lucia Grimaldi, Department of Romance Philology, Freie Universität Berlin,
Germany.
INTRODUCTION
The book focuses on two phenomena which supposedly occur in the
formation of comparative constructions: comparative deletion (CD) and
comparative ellipsis (CE). The author develops an analysis of comparatives
which does not require construction-specific deletion processes. He claims
that the phenomena generally attributed to CD and CE, stem from
movement and from more general deletion processes. All mechanisms
involved should be located in syntax. The book is divided into five sections:
Introduction, Comparative Deletion, Comparative Ellipsis, Comparative
Coordination, and Conclusion.
SYNOPSIS
The first chapter (Introduction, 1-7) includes a short outline of the main
claims and introduces the terms "comparative deletion" and "comparative
ellipsis". Comparative deletion (CD) denotes a deletion process first
described by Bresnan (1973). Assuming that in comparative constructions
two degrees are involved, CD obligatorily triggers the deletion of the
second degree/adjectival phrase:
(1) Bill is taller than John is [CD] (CD = tall)
Generally a distinction is made between CD and another deletion
mechanism, so-called comparative ellipsis (CE) (cf. among others Pinkham
1982), by which phrasal comparatives (PC) are derived from full sentential
counterparts. This ellipsis process is optional and applies in addition to CD,
deleting further constituents under identity with the matrix clause:
(2) Bill is taller than John [CE] [CD] (CE = is, CD = tall)
In chapter 2 (Comparative Deletion, 9-88), the author tackles the CD
phenomenon, discussing the precise nature of the process. In line with
numerous approaches, he postulates a comparative structure in which two
degree phrases (DegPs) are present, with Deg as a functional category
taking AP as an argument (cf. Abney 1987). However, he does not locate the
AP in the complement, but rather in the Specifier of the DegP. The than-XP
is base-generated in the complement position of the first degree phrase. In
the case of a synthetic comparative, the Deg-head, otherwise filled
by "more", is phonetically empty. It is characterized by the feature
[+comparative], which has to be checked by an AP in a Spec/Head
configuration. The comparative structure postulated by the author is the
following (p. 22):
(3) Bill is [DegP [AP taller] [Deg' [Deg [+comparative]][XP than John is [DegP
[AP [CD]]]]]]
In addition, operator movement is involved in the comparative clause; this is
a widely accepted assumption since Chomsky (1977). The empty operator
(OP) is raised to SpecCP and binds the Deg-Trace in the complement
position of the DegP (p. 38):
(4) Bill is taller than [CP OP(i) [IP John is [DegP [AP [CD]] [Deg' Deg t(i)]]]]
NP-comparatives with attributive modifier (in the following called:
attributive comparatives) pose a classical problem in all analyses that
assume the than-XP to be generated within the first DegP:
(5) I know younger authors than Peter knows
Under the common assumption that prenominal adjectives are left adjoined
to NP, the comparative structure in (3) would yield the following wrong
linearization:
(6) *I know younger than Peter knows authors
In order to avoid (6), the than-XP either has to be extraposed to the right,
or else another analysis for prenominal attributives has to be chosen. The
author takes the second option and, in line with Abney (1987), opts for a
DP-structure in which the AP is selected by the D-head, and takes the NP as
a complement (p. 29):
(7) [D [AP young [NP authors ]]]
Starting with (3) and (7) as basis the author proposes the following analysis
for (5):
(8) I know [DP [DegP [AP younger [NP authors]][Deg' [Deg [+comparative]]
[XP than Peter knows [CD]]]]]
To turn back to the question regarding the precise nature of the process
described as CD, the author suggests that the AP within the than-XP should
be raised to the matrix clause (10), a solution inspired by the Raising
Analysis of the relative clause (cf. Bianchi 1999) shown in (9):
(9) [DP the [CP authors(i) [C [IP I know t(i)]]]]
(10) [DP [DegP [better authors](i) than [CP I know [DP [DegP t(i)]]]]] (t = CD
= good authors)
CD would thus not be a deletion process but rather a special case of
movement, AP-raising. The movement is motivated by the Deg-head of the
matrix clause which, as mentioned above, is characterized by the feature
[+comparative] that needs to be checked by AP-raising (p. 50). Due to the
specific assumptions on AP-structure in attributive comparatives, the NP is
subject to pied-piping, thus being also affected by movement. With
reference to Poole (1996), the author claims that in AP-raising no chain
formation is involved. As a consequence, both copies remain visible for
semantics, whereas in the usual movement scenario only one copy from the
movement chain is in fact interpreted semantically (p. 87).
With regards to the representational level of CD, the author argues against
approaches which assume that the constituent affected by CD is only
reconstructed on the level of semantic interpretation. He lists some
phenomena in support of the evidence that the CD-constituent is visible on
logical form (LF) and thus must be a result of syntactic derivation processes.
In Chapter 3 (Comparative Ellipsis, 89-183) the author presents data in
support of the view that reduced comparatives such as (13) and reduced
coordinations such as (14) obey similar restrictions, e.g. prohibiting the
gapping of a verb with an embedded finite CP (p. 100) (in the following,
deleted elements shall be placed in square brackets):
(13) *Lisa promised that her mother will visit Millhouse and Sally [promised
that her mother will visit] Otto
(14) *More people promised that their friends will visit Millhouse than [CD]
[promised that their friends will visit] Otto
The only difference is that in (13) the second subject is overt whereas it is
eliminated by AP-raising in (14). As a consequence, the author opts for a
unified analysis of reduced comparatives and reduced coordination. Both
are assumed as being derived from full clausal constructions by conjunction
reduction (CR) operations, especially gapping, right node raising and across
the board movement. This claim, called Conjunction Reduction (CR)-
Hypothesis (p. 114) implies that a construction-specific ellipsis rule such as
comparative ellipsis can be dispensed with.
The author next turns to phrasal comparatives (PCs) which are defined as
comparatives with one (non-verbal) remnant in the than-XP, as opposed to
partially reduced comparatives (PRCs), which include more than one
remnant in the than-XP (p.89). He presents a large variety of English and
German data to support evidence that PCs and PRCs obey the same internal
and external restrictions. Since PRCs are uncontroversially analyzed as
derived from sentential comparatives, consequently PCs too should be
treated as derived structures. This conclusion is clearly formulated in the
PC-Hypothesis: "All PCs without explicit standards derive from a clausal
source" (p. 93). It follows that all comparatives should be analyzed in the
same fashion, i.e. in terms of AP-raising. Additionally, PCs and PRCs are
subject to ellipsis operations, previously identified as CR operations.
A further assumption regarding the structure of PCs and PRCs should be
mentioned here: the author posits a so-called comparative coordination,
according to which comparatives are optionally parsed as coordinate
structures (p. 110). This property only affects reduced comparatives and is
linked to the application of CR operations. It is also limited to the syntactic
derivation: it is not reflected in the semantics of the comparative clause
which is assumed to be semantically subordinated (p. 224). Comparative
coordination explains data such as (15) in which the subjects of the matrix
and the than-clause have different agreement features (p. 162):
(15) weil wir mehr Buecher gekauft haben als Peter [CD] gekauft
hatbecause we more books bought have than Peter bought has
While the application of gapping is possible, giving (15)a, right node raising
does not seem to be acceptable, since it would lead to the ungrammatical
(15)b (deleted elements in square brackets):
(15)a. weil wir mehr Buecher gekauft haben als Peter [[CD] gekauft hat]
because we more books bought have than Peter bought has
(15)b. *weil wir mehr Buecher [gekauft haben] als Peter [CD] gekauft
hatbecause we more books bought have than Peter bought has
It appears that in comparatives where subjects with different personal
features are involved, the remaining verb has to agree with the subject of
the matrix clause. The author proposes a solution to this problem by
claiming that the height of coordination can differ in reduced comparatives.
Assuming a finer grained IP-structure, the coordination may apply at VP/vP,
AspP, TP or AgrSP (p. 166). In cases such as (15) the coordination applies at
an early stage in the derivation, at the VP-level. This means that there is
only one TP available, which contains an auxiliary and the subject of the
matrix clause. Therefore the auxiliary obligatorily agrees with the matrix
subject. The subject of the than-clause remains in situ and can't have
influence on verbal morphology (the case-filter is satisfied by default case).
Thus (15)b is ruled out.
The fourth chapter (Comparative Coordination, 185-227) takes a closer
look at the assumed coordinate structure for comparatives. The author
claims that "Initially, the than-XP is base-generated in the complement
position of the degree head, resulting in a subordinate parse" (p. 190). In
cases, in which the than-XP is on the right periphery of the whole
construction, a particular extraposition operation, so-called than-XP
raising (TR), is assumed to have applied. This operation is optional in
principle, but becomes obligatory, in the case of a CR operation requiring
*Embedding, e.g. gapping. TR, which is only available in comparatives, and
not in coordination, explains why comparatives such as (16) can have wide
and narrow ellipsis reading in a and b respectively, while coordination
cannot, as is shown in (17) (p. 186s.):
(16) John wants to write more plays than Sam [e] [CD]
a. [e = wants to write]
b. [e = writes/wrote]
(17) John wanted to write plays and Sam [e] poems
a. [e = wanted to write]
b. *[e = wrote/writes]
The claim is that, when two IPs are involved in the matrix clause, the than-
XP is attached at different levels of the structure, i.e. at the higher IP in (16)a
and at the lower IP in (16)b. This option is not available in coordination
where no TR is involved. In the last part of chapter four, the structure of
comparative coordination is modified (p. 225): instead of a three-branch-
representation, a binary structure is favoured, this fully in line
with "asymmetric" analyses of coordination.
Chapter 5 (Conclusion, p. 229-233) contains a brief summary of the main
claims and cites some still unresolved problems. It is followed by the Notes,
the Bibliography as well as a Subject and an Author Index.
EVALUATION
The book is a synthesis of Lechner (1999) and Lechner (2001)
supplemented with some recent considerations. It provides the reader with
an impressive variety of interesting data concerning sentential and phrasal
comparatives in German and English and also presents some appealing
ideas, such as taking up the Raising Analysis of relative clauses and
applying it to comparatives (cf. Donati 1997, 2000 for a different
elaboration of the same concept). The resolution of comparative deletion as
syntactic movement, i.e. AP-raising, solves some problems related with
semantic and phonological ellipsis accounts of CD. In contrast to semantic
approaches, it does not assume that the CD-site has either empty or no
syntactic structure at all, and thus avoids problems related with scope and
binding properties of the CD-site. Since only the AP(+NP) is targeted by AP-
raising and by subsequent phonological deletion of the lower copy, an
identity condition between the two degree heads, generally required by
phonological deletion accounts, becomes superfluous.
The PC-Hypothesis, which assumes all comparative complements to be
derived from full thematic clauses, has the theoretical advantage of being
semantically transparent, whereas the so-called direct analyses, which
analyze the comparative complement as a simple prepositional phrase, have
to explain how the missing structure can be reconstructed semantically (cf.
Kennedy 1999: p.151). Additionally, the PC-Hypothesis reduces the number
of phenomena to be dealt with, a desirable consequence from the point of
view of economy. The author presents interesting arguments on how to
solve classical problems of a derived analysis of phrasal comparatives.
These are shown by the following data, in which a clausal basis for the
derivation does not seem to be available:
(18) John is older than me [*am] (p. 179)
(19) John(i) couldn't possibly be taller than himself(i) [*is] (p. 180)
(20) She ran faster than the world record [*ran] (p. 182)
The author remarks that the accusative case in (18) is not an exclusive
property of PCs, but can also be found in reduced coordination, an
additional argument for the CR-Hypothesis, at least for English data:
(21) John is eager to see the movies, and me too (p. 180)
The ungrammaticality of (19) is explained by assuming that a small clause
(SC) taking the remnant as a subject is the basis of the derivation.
According to this analysis the structure of the than-XP is [-finite] and no
binding principle is violated. The same small clause analysis is assumed for
(20). But this case is somewhat problematic, since the assumption of an
underlying structure implies that CD has occurred. This means that the
derivational basis of (20) would be something like (22), which seems
problematic for a semantic interpretation:
(22) #She ran faster than [SC the world record [CD fast]]
Furthermore, there is good reason of being critical towards the strong
distinction made between the PC-Hypothesis and direct analyses. While the
author uses the number of remnant constituents as a discerning criterion
between PCs (one remnant) and PRCs (more than one) (p. 89), this is not
necessarily the defining criterion for direct analyses. In these theories only a
subset of the constructions defined as PCs by the author are considered as
base generated (cf. among others Hankamer 1973, Napoli 1983). These
include the problematic cases in (18)-(20) and comparatives in which the
remnant is compared to the subject of the matrix clause (a more precise
definition is not possible in this context). In this more narrow sense, base
generated PCs are distinguished from other non-sentential comparatives.
Note that this distinction is realized by different particles in some languages
(e.g. in Italian). Taking into account this reduced applicability of direct
analyses, and considering that the PC-Hypothesis cannot resolve all
problematic cases (cf. fn. 145, p. 258), and excludes "subcomparatives,
comparatives with explicit standards and small clause comparatives" (cf. fn.
177, p. 261), the distinction between the PC-Hypothesis and direct analyses
doesn't seem so clear cut anymore.
A similar remark can be made with regards to the strong CR-Hypothesis:
The claim that no construction specific ellipsis operation (such as
comparative ellipsis) is required is well motivated. However, the stronger
claim that all reduced comparatives can be explained by CR operations
seems too radical, a fact which is partly recognized by the author (cf. fn.
105, p. 250) and partly shown by examples with IP- and CP-ellipsis, which
violate at least the principle of Isomorphism (deleted elements in square
brackets):
(23) John is taller than I expected [IP John to be [CD]]
(24)
Jan ist groesser als ich dachte [CP dass Jan [CD] ist]
John is taller than I thought that John is
An advantage of the present proposal consists in the assumed DegP-
structure. On the one hand, it yields the right linearization without
obligatory extraposition of the than-XP (cf. Bresnan 1973). Furthermore, it
avoids another well known problem for the analysis of comparatives,
namely Left Branch Extraction (cf. Corver 1990). However, there is a
problem related to the structure of attributive comparatives. As the author
points out, the [AP[NP]]-nexus shows conflicting categorial properties: in
order to fulfil the requirements of AP-raising it must be detected as an AP.
However, being a verbal argument headed by a DP, it requires the external
structure of an NP. As a solution to this conflict, the author introduces a
functional category (FP) which takes the NP as a complement and the AP as
a specifier. While only the AP is targeted by AP-raising, it is the whole FP,
including the NP, which is pied-piped. According to this analysis the
category affected by AP-raising is no longer an AP but an FP with the
categorial status of NP (p. 45s.). It is not clear how the Spec/Head-
configuration required for the checking of the [+comparative]-feature in
Deg and for the phonological realization of comparative morphology (p. 23)
may be established in a structure of this kind.
One of the merits of this book lies in the idea of treating comparatives as
hybrid structures located between subordination and coordination. It allows
for an explanation of similarities with relatives (cf. Chomsky 1977; Donati
1997, 2000) as well as with coordinate structures (cf. Napoli 1983).
Nonetheless, the argument concerning comparative coordination is
somewhat circular. On the one hand the author stipulates a "coordinate
structure required in order to provide the syntactic basis for the application
of CR" (p. 232s.). In this case the coordinate structure seems to be the
prerequisite for CR operations. But in chapter 3, CR operations, specifically
across the board (ATB) movement, are taken to be the condition required
for the coordinate structure, and the causal chain seems to be
inverted: "Whenever comparative formation implicates an ATB movement
process such as ATB V2, the structure has to be parsed as a comparative
coordination" and, even more explicitly: "... ATB movement triggers a
comparative coordination...." (p. 172).
The sequence of arguments is generally clear, with one exception: the
structure of comparative coordination, including TR and the asymmetric
analysis of the coordinate structure, is introduced only in chapter 4; yet, it
would have been helpful in chapter 3 for a better comprehension of
intraposed and extraposed PCs (3.3.4) and of the restrictions on
comparative coordination (3.4).
Although one of the declared goals of the analysis was to avoid structure
dependent mechanisms such as comparative deletion and comparative
ellipsis, the author is forced to introduce some "unorthodox" mechanisms,
such as movement without chain, switch between subordinate and
coordinate parse, and obligatory TR. There seems to be no escape: one
either makes a finer grained distinction of the phenomena at stake - a
solution partly chosen by the author by excluding subcomparatives from his
account -, or alternatively one has to assume construction-specific
mechanisms.
In conclusion, despite the problems discussed so far, the book is an
important contribution to the comprehension of these complex
constructions and should be taken into account for further research.
REFERENCES
Abney, Steven (1987): The English Noun Phrase in its Sentential Aspect.
Ph.D. diss., Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, MIT.
Bianchi, Valentina (1999): Consequences of Antisymmetry: Headed Relative
Clauses. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Bresnan, Joan (1973): "Syntax of the comparative clause construction in
English", Linguistic Inquiry 4, 275-343.
Chomsky, Noam (1977): "On Wh-movement". In: Peter Culicover, Tom
Wasow and Adrian Akmajian (eds.): Formal Syntax, 71-132. New York:
Academic Press.
Corver, Norbert (1990): The Syntax of Left Branch Extraction. Ph.D. diss.,
Department of Linguistics, University of Tilburg.
Donati, Caterina (1997): "Comparative clauses as free relatives: A raising
analysis", Probus 9, 145-166.
Donati, Caterina (2000): La sintassi della comparazione. Padova: Unipress.
Hankamer, Jorge (1973): "Why There are two than's in English", CLS 9, 179-
191.
Kennedy, Christopher (1999): Projecting the adjective. The syntax and
semantics of gradability and comparison. New York/London: Garland.
Lechner, Winfried (1999): Comparatives and DP-structure. Ph.D. diss.,
University of Massachusetts.
Lechner, Winfried (2001): "Reduced and Phrasal Comparatives", Natural
Language and Linguistic Theory 19, 683-735.
Napoli, Donna Jo (1983): "Comparative Ellipsis: A Phrase Structure Analysis",
Linguistic Inquiry 14, 675-694.
Pinkham, Jessie (1982): The Formation of Comparative Clauses in French
and English. Ph.D. diss., Cambridge MA: Harvard University.
Poole, Geoffrey (1996): "Optional Movement in the Minimalist Program", in:
Werner Abraham, Samuel Epstein, Höskuldur Thráinsson and Jan-Wouter
Zwart (eds.): Minimal Ideas. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 199-
216.
ABOUT THE REVIEWER
Lucia Grimaldi is a teaching and research assistant for Italian Linguistics at
the Freie Universität Berlin. Her field of research (Ph.D. dissertation in
progress) covers the syntax of comparative constructions and related
phenomena (wh-movement, DP structure, focus, syntax and semantics of
degree). She mainly works on Italian and other Romance languages.
-----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-16-599
More information about the LINGUIST
mailing list