17.1055, Review: Typology: Haspelmath, Dryer, Gil & Comrie (2005)
linguist at LINGUISTLIST.ORG
linguist at LINGUISTLIST.ORG
Fri Apr 7 23:51:07 UTC 2006
LINGUIST List: Vol-17-1055. Fri Apr 07 2006. ISSN: 1068 - 4875.
Subject: 17.1055, Review: Typology: Haspelmath, Dryer, Gil & Comrie (2005)
Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Wayne State U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>
Reviews (reviews at linguistlist.org)
Sheila Dooley, U of Arizona
Terry Langendoen, U of Arizona
Homepage: http://linguistlist.org/
The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University, Wayne
State University, and donations from subscribers and publishers.
Editor for this issue: Lindsay Butler <lindsay at linguistlist.org>
================================================================
This LINGUIST List issue is a review of a book published by one of our
supporting publishers, commissioned by our book review editorial staff. We
welcome discussion of this book review on the list, and particularly invite
the author(s) or editor(s) of this book to join in. To start a discussion of
this book, you can use the Discussion form on the LINGUIST List website. For
the subject of the discussion, specify "Book Review" and the issue number of
this review. If you are interested in reviewing a book for LINGUIST, look for
the most recent posting with the subject "Reviews: AVAILABLE FOR REVIEW", and
follow the instructions at the top of the message. You can also contact the
book review staff directly.
===========================Directory==============================
1)
Date: 31-Mar-2006
From: Mark Donohue < mark at donohue.cc >
Subject: The World Atlas of Language Structures
-------------------------Message 1 ----------------------------------
Date: Fri, 07 Apr 2006 19:46:20
From: Mark Donohue < mark at donohue.cc >
Subject: The World Atlas of Language Structures
Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/16/16-3425.html
EDITORS: Haspelmath, Martin; Dryer, Matthew S.; Gil, David; Comrie,
Bernard
TITLE: The World Atlas of Language Structures
PUBLISHER: Oxford University Press
YEAR: 2005
Mark Donohue, Monash University and National University of
Singapore
OVERVIEW
The World Atlas of Language Structures (WALS) is a lot of fun. Before
anything else, that needs to be said. Addictively much fun. Beware!
This is not simply because of the interesting subject matter that it
deals with, but also because of the inclusion of a CD-ROM version of
the atlas; though calling the CD a 'version' of the atlas is unfair, and it
would be more accurate to call the large atlas a version of the CD-
ROM. But more on the specifics of the CD-ROM later.
WALS is an ambitious undertaking, and represents the first realisation
of a research program that will, no doubt, change the way we do
linguistics. This is the first attempt to map a large number of linguistic
features (142 different features, ranging from the phonetic,
phonological, morphological to the lexical and the paralinguistic) found
in a large number of languages (the core sample is 200 languages; see
http://linguistics.buffalo.edu/people/faculty/dryer/dryer/atlas.map200.html
for the list). Nichols (1992) is the only comparable published work
in terms of aims and scope, and that deals with 174 languages, and
10 multivalued linguistic features, all morphological. To say that WALS
significantly increases the degree to which linguists are exposed to
typological mapping is to make a great understatement.
The four editors coordinated the work of 54 chapter authors, each of
whom produced a map (or maps) with an explanatory mini-chapter
describing one aspect of linguistic structure (see the end of this review
for the full list). The choice of which features were included on the
maps is one that cannot, of course, satisfy everyone, though I must
admit I did find most of my reasonable hopes fulfilled. (I have a number
of unreasonable hopes, the lack of fulfillment of which I do not
begrudge the editors.) A full list of the (sometimes multivalued)
features included in the atlas can be found at the end of this review.
On the other hand, the presence of some of the maps that made it into
WALS confuses me. Why was the presence of an affricate or a plain
stop as the onset in the word for 'tea' deemed map-worthy? If we
really wanted to know, we could, as the author of the map admits he
did in many (most?) cases, got to http://www.travlang.com and look up
the word. The map plots the lexemes used for 'tea' in 230 languages,
arranging them by stop-initial 'tea'-like words, affricate-initial 'cha'-like
words, and others, attributing the stop-initial versions to an ultimate
Min origin in China (the author mentions Amoy as the origin of the
etymon, but curiously does not include a language reference from this
area, the closest being found on Taiwan. The absence of any attempt
to indicate intermediate loci of diffusion, and the astonishing lack of
analysis of many of the forms found in languages. For example,
Soninke _dute_ is listed as one of the 'others' (not derivable
from 'Sinitic _cha_' or 'Min Nan Chinese _te55_' (_te55_ is a Chaozhou
[=Teochew] form; why it, and not an Amoy form, is cited is left
unexplained). The most cursory analysis would reveal a French
etymology for the Soninke word (<_du thée_), entirely plausible given
the colonial history of Mali. Similarly Hawaiian _kii_ is given as
an 'other', ignoring the well-known *k > t sound change that has
applied in Hawaiian, revealing a minimally-changed English loanword.
The author notes that in English some dialects preserve a form
spelled as 'tay', reflecting an 'older' pronunciation; but why not also
mention northern English 'char', as in char-lady, reflecting the
alternative non-Min etymology and not particularly hard to find out
about?
The reasons for the inclusion of this particular piece of 'language
structure' with this lack of analysis can only be known to the editors.
Another issue I have with the editing is the lack of consistency
between chapters; this is hailed as a sign of the vitality of the atlas
('no attempt was made to make chapters by different authors that
overlap in their features consistent' - from the Introduction), but to this
reviewer it feels more like a lack of editorial authority: what, other than
deciding on the languages and language features, was the role of the
editors, if not to ensure consistency across different chapters?
To answer my own question, another role that I would assume fell to
the editors was one of language selection, and typological
representation. The core set of languages chosen clearly reflects
careful thought, and is a set of languages that shows both areal and
genetic diversity (it is, however, remarkably hard to make a map of all
and only these 200 languages).
Many of the maps, however, clearly do not reflect adherence to the
WALS list of core languages at all, and are simply the authors' own
private databases made into publishable form. The maps of colour
terms, for instance, show no evidence of having consulted publicly-
available dictionaries or lexicons of the languages in the sample, and
as such does not contain enough information to really justify two
foolscap pages of mapping: there simply isn't enough information (119
languages) to be able to make areal judgements (there are, for
instance, five entries for all of Australia, and three for all of mainland
east Asia [Mandarin, Korean and Japanese]). Here, too, there has
been no attempt to enforce the language 'minimum' (for more on the
number of languages represented, see below).
Most of the features are, however, the types of things that linguists
deal with every day: unusual consonants, size of the vowel inventory,
alignment (of 'full' NPs or pronouns), word order, demonstrative
contrasts, etc.). What is there to be gained by displaying these
features on a map? To answer this question, and to fully appreciate
WALS it is necessary to 'try it out'; short of purchasing it and poring
over it for days, interested readers can examine some samples pages
and maps on the Max Planck website (http://www.eva.mpg.de/lingua/files/wals.html).
This doesn't let you appreciate how useful it can be to correlate a
pair of features, which you have customised into the values that you
want, and spot the patterns (this is possible with the CD). It does,
however, allow you to see the wealth of information that some of the
'fuller' maps contain. And, more to the point, it allows you to appreciate
the geographical distribution of these features; a surprising number of
linguistic features show strong areal or macro-areal tendencies, cutting
across any genetic boundaries in their way. We have all known that there
are linguistic areas (such as South Asia, the Balkans, North America's
Pacific Northwest) in which languages converge towards a pan-
genetic norm; but with WALS we can see just _how_ prevalent areal
clusterings are in the distribution of linguistic structure.
THE DATA
This is not a criticism of WALS directly, or even of typology specifically,
but of any research that relies on secondary sources, and it is
obvious. Any research that relies on secondary sources can only be
as accurate as the reporting in the primary sources and as the
interpretation that the secondary author has of those primary sources.
As I said, this is obvious. The problems, however, are magnified when
undertaking a project that aims to have a minimum of 200 languages
on each of 142 maps.
The WALS organisers apparently recognised this, and requested of
the primary source authors that they would be available to answer any
questions of interpretation. This is an admirable safety measure, as
anyone who has browsed through a grammar knows that the data one
is looking for can sometimes be hard to find, can be spread out over
different sections with only indifferent cross-referencing, or even
completely absent. It can only be effective, however, if it is used. As
one of the authors of a grammar on the 200-language core sample I
was contacted less than a handful of times by a WALS author. This
might reflect superb clarity in the Tukang Besi grammar (I can only
hope), but the fact that Tukang Besi, a core language, is missing from
7 maps shows that that isn't true. I cannot possibly examine the
representation of every feature for every language in detail, so I will
concentrate on a few areas for which I have expertise. Even taking
into account the explanatory chapterettes, I took exception to the
representation of Tukang Besi no less than 20 of the 142 maps, 14%
of the total (the list can be found at [http://www.donohue.cc], then
following the link to WALS). I do not know how well this reflects the
accuracy of the maps as a whole; but I managed to find a large
number of encoded decisions which I strongly disagreed with (some of
which are mentioned at the end of this review). If we assume that
approximately 10% of the information encoded is inaccurate, then the
maps with small numbers of languages become highly
unrepresentative, and even the larger maps need to be approached
with caution.
THE SIZE AND COVERAGE OF THE MAPS
Some of the maps abound in information; some do not. In general,
those maps that have 300 languages or more look nicely 'full'; the
occasional map that hits the 1,000 language mark (Dryer's maps of
word order patterns) are especially satisfying (though I am not sure
how a word order was assigned to many of the languages described
as having free order of words in a clause, with no evidence for NP
constituents; Warlpiri, for instance, has adjective-noun order, and
Dyirbal is noun-adjective; how was this determined in the absence of
NPs? Dryer (p371) mentions text counts as a determiner, siding with
Greenberg for frequency (and against Dryer 1995; though note that the
maps plot _dominant_ word order, and not basic word order), but in
the Dyirbal text I checked there were no instances of modificational
adjectives). On the other hand there are quite a few maps that have
obvious gaps, and so are not representative. This is particularly
apparent if you examine a small area of the world (the CD comes with
built-in 'zooms' of Australia, the Caucasus, and Indonesia; it is simple
to create your own default zoom areas). When you zoom to an area
5000km long and find two dots, you do not get a representative view
of that feature in that part of the world. In some cases this lack reflects
a genuine lack of carefully assembled data; and it might be that even
the 200 language 'core' sample lacks easily findable information on a
particular topic. On the other hand, it was for this very purpose that
various language experts were contacted and asked if they would
mind being questioned by some of the compilers. Similarly, there are
some topics that are relatively easily researched (such as relative
clauses) and which are not very satisfyingly represented.
The number of entries on a map of a particular feature varies wildly.
As stated above, the authors attempted to include a 'core sample' of
200 languages on every map, with authors encouraged to include
other languages. Many maps do not meet the 200 language minimum
target; this strikes this reviewer as an issue with which the editors
should have engaged a little more firmly with the authors. On the other
hand, other maps display over 1,000 different languages. The
promotional material states that 'Each world map shows an average of
400 languages'; this is true, but the median number of languages per
map is 301. As anyone familiar with statistics knows, if the mean and
the median differ radically, as is true here, it suggests that we are not
dealing with one population. Indeed, most of the more 'full' maps are
the work of three authors, Matthew Dryer, Ian Maddieson and Cecil
Brown. If these authors' maps are excluded from the count we find the
average and median both drop to 250 languages per map. This
means that only just over half the maps have that 'full' look.
THE CD
The interactive CD version of WALS was implemented by Hans-Jörg
Bibiko, who deserves a huge thanks from the linguistics community. In
a real sense the CD is the publication, and the large, hard-bound,
attractive atlas is simply a by-product of the CD.
All the information necessary to produce the atlas can be found in the
CD (with a couple of exceptions - the map for writing systems, for
instance, is not available on the CD). Through the us of the CD the
user is also capable of customising maps. This can apply to trivial
things (You don't like that shade of brown? Change it to yellow! Fed
up with little diamond figures on the map? Make them squares!), but it
can also make for quite significant changes: You can't see the point in
distinguishing between value 3 and value 4 in a map? Combine them!
This allows you to simplify maps to display just the value, or cluster of
values, that you want for a particular grammatical feature. But even
more interestingly, it is possible to combine features together. For
instance, if we are interested in OV versus VO order in main clauses,
and wish to see if there are correlations with the presence of tone, we
can create a map combining these two grammatical features, and
plotting the intersection of the values for the relative clause feature
with those of the word order feature. This will create a bewilderingly
complex map; so we simplify it: not interested in languages with no
dominant order in the clause? Take'em off. Not interested in the
difference between 'pitch-accent' and 'full tone' languages? Combine
them. Now we have an interpretable map (I made this map; in fact, I
made several maps showing the intersection of various subsets of
these features. You can look at them by going to
http://www.donohue.cc, and following the WALS link). And we find
that, of the 383 languages that meet the combined criteria, roughly
half of the VO languages show tonal behaviour, while only one quarter
of the OV languages show tone. Furthermore, VO languages with
tones and OV languages with tone are generally adjacent (the
exception being the OV tonal languages of New Guinea), implying that
tone is an areal feature independent of the order of the verb and its
object: there is no correlation between the order of a verb and its
object with the presence of tone.
ANY of the features in WALS can be so combined (though only two
features at a time, in the current version). This allows for us to check
both the possible, but silly combinations (tone and verb-object order)
and some of the sensible ones (tone and syllable structure, for
instance). And who knows? With enough people trying enough 'silly'
combinations, we might well learn about which ones are sensible, and
might well be surprised. One disadvantage of the compiling feature is
the fact that, as mentioned before, commonality between languages
plotted on different maps is elusive. As a result, there is only a small
set of languages that are guaranteed to be shared between two
maps, with the subsequent result that maps of combinations can yield
very small outputs, unless you make sure you only take the numerous-
language maps as your inputs.
Nonetheless, the CD is wonderful. The ability to zoom into small
areas, to select exactly which features you want to view, and how to
view them, is likely to be the cause of many missed deadlines (and
hopefully several new and intriguing discoveries). The CD should,
ideally, be marketed as a separate teaching tool. The amount that can
be taught through the hands-on use of this software, about typology
and the distribution of language patterns across the world, is
immense. Unfortunately the current all-inclusive price for the atlas +
CD prohibits this.
Some other comments on the electronic WALS:
* Some menus insist on being in German; I have learned the words
Schreibtisch, sichern, and something else (meaning, I
gather, 'cancel'). Learning some German isn't a _terrible_ thing, but it
is, shall we say, an unexpected and insistent bonus.
* The CD-ROM will not scale font sizes to screen; you should set your
screen's resolution to a low value before working with WALS, unless
your eyesight is both very good, and unstrainable.
* The _topology_ and _ocean_ map option features are not
independent of each other; something's not working.
* Some important geographic features are missing - the Sepik river
does not appear, for instance.
* When using the language-name-on-scroll-over with a map that
employs expanded language dots, the scroll-over applies over an
area much, much greater than the dot.
* 'right click for editing' ?? - what does this mean for a Macintosh user?
* I have found exactly one typo, which is a pretty remarkable
achievement. In the CD version, if you take 'the tour' (a demonstrative
user guide) you come across one point where you see 'five differnet
values'.
* On the CD, long strings of words are cut off from the map legend
when using the feature composer and collapsing distinctions between
different values of a feature (for instance, examining syllable types
and the size of a consonant inventory, I created 'Complex AND Small
OR Moderately small OR Average', which fits into the space allotted
for a map legend, but nothing longer will.
SOME GENERAL CRITIQUES.
Some of the genetic affiliations used in the classification are
controversial, though none are ones that are reviled by reasonable
linguists. Examples of these include 'Australian' and 'Trans New
Guinea', used with various degrees of acceptance
CONTROVERSIAL MAPPING DECISIONS
The dot indicating the position of Ekari (a western New Guinea
language) is a long way to the west of where I would have placed it.
Nara (in Ethiopia) appears to be in Eritrea on the maps. Kiwai is a lot
further north than I would have expected. Mangap-Mbula is not
spoken in the Vitiaz straits, as appears on the maps.
CONTROVERSIAL CODING DECISIONS:
Is there a gender distinction in 3sg pronouns in Mandarin? In the
written form, yes, but both are pronounced [tha]. If we want to go by
written forms it is also true that subtitles in Mandarin have a gender
distinction for 2sg as well. This subtitling distinction of feminine and
non-feminine has not spread to other writing domains as yet, but it
does exist. Are we deliberately excluding subtitling from our
considerations? It seems so, but on what reasoned grounds. And,
since most of the languages in the world lack (self-innovated) writing
systems, surely we should look at speech when comparing things?
The voiced stops in Thai are at least partly implosive, and minimally
deserve comment, if not necessarily coding as part of the set of
glottalised consonants.
I have documented a large number of what I consider to be coding
errors in WALS; I have conducted an exhaustive check of the codings
for Tukang Besi, and, as mentioned earlier, found that there is a 14%
error rate in the coding of the data. A list of the coding decisions I
disagree with, stated briefly, can be found at [http://www.donohue.cc],
and then following the WALS link. I have, along the way, compiled a
list of my objections to the coding decisions made for other languages,
necessarily less complete and with less authority, but nonetheless with
a significant number of entries. These examples, too, are available
online.
OVERALL COMMENTS
I tend towards being a perfectionist, and so I have picked at the flaws
which appeared to me to be most salient in WALS - and they are not
insignificant. But we should remember that this is the first edition of the
first volume heralding a whole new research program: patience! This
is something that is different enough that we should be
accommodating of some teething problems.
Can you ignore the WALS? No. Can you believe it as it stands? In
broad outline, yes; but for the details, no. The WALS maps have the
feel of the first release of a Microsoft product: useful, colourful, rather
exciting, but basically being beta-tested on the paying public. Knowing
how to interpret these broad outlines is also something that linguists
are not, in the main, well-trained in, and the experience of critically
assessing these data will be useful. Good linguists make a career out
of learning to interpret data, and carefully checking their sources.
WALS is not a shortcut that allows you to avoid that, but it certainly
enables a person to very quickly know which data to steer towards.
What's the bottom line on WALS, as I see it? I've had access to a copy
for 6 weeks now, and it _has_ changed the way I think of, and do,
linguistics, in a good way. I am currently writing an article that has
changed dramatically as a result of my access to WALS. Based on
this, I have no hesitation in saying that everyone should have access
to a copy of WALS. Should they rush out and buy it right now?
Contradictorily, no. There are enough easily-fixed mistakes and
omissions in the current version of WALS that a second edition should
be a quite marked improvement on the current first edition, and should
be available without too much delay. In effect, the current version of
WALS is being beta-tested on the wider audience. Should everyone
encourage their libraries to get a copy now? Without hesitation of
qualm of any kind, yes. This is a hold-in-your-hands demonstration of
a methodology that has been growing (see, for example, Bickel and
Nichols 2002), and is now available to be examined by anyone. This is
world-wide typology waiting to happen without having to own, or even
borrow, hundreds of books. This is a very real chance to spot
correlations between different linguistic features, between linguistic
features and areas, and between linguistic features and language
families. And, to return to the start of this review: it's a _lot_ of fun.
REFERENCES
Bickel, Balthasar, and Nichols, Johanna. 2002ff. The Autotyp research
program.http://www.uni-leipzig.de/~autotyp/
Dryer, Matthew S. 1995. Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word
order. In Pamela Downing and Michael Noonan, eds., Word order in
discourse: 105-135. Typological Studies in Language 30. Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Nichols, Johanna. 1992. Linguistic diversity in space and time.
University of Chicago Press.
ABOUT THE REVIEWER
Mark Donohue is primarily a syntactician, but is also very interested in
modelling tone and epenthesis, as well as the work and methodology
of historical linguistics. Initially working on languages and language
relationships in Southeast Sulawesi, he has also investigated
Austronesian languages from Flores and northern New Guinea, as
well as working on non-Austronesian languages of the Timor-Alor-
Pantar group, the Western Ok and Kwerba families, the Dani family,
the Skou family, the West-Papuan family and the Torricelli family. The
languages he has worked on all share the property of being on the
very edge of a genetic unit or units: they show the effects of language
contact and unusual grammaticalisation. Mark has published a
grammar of Tukang Besi and sketches of Warembori and I'saka, and
is currently preparing a grammar of Skou and a formal grammar of
Tukang Besi. When he finds the time, he intends to finish his
monograph on the use of lexical transitivity patterns as a comparative
tool.
-----------------------------------------------------------
This Year the LINGUIST List hopes to raise $52,932. This money will go to help keep the
List running by supporting all of our Student Editors for the coming year.
See below for donation instructions, and don't forget to check out our Fund Drive 2006
LINGUIST List Cruise for some Fund Drive fun!
http://linguistlist.org/cruise.html
There are many ways to donate to LINGUIST!
You can donate right now using our secure credit card form.
Alternatively you can also pledge right now and pay later.
For all information on donating and pledging, including information on how to donate by
check, money order, or wire transfer, please visit:
http://linguistlist.org/donate.html
The LINGUIST List is under the umbrella of Eastern Michigan University and as such can
receive donations through the EMU Foundation, which is a registered 501(c) Non Profit
organization. Our Federal Tax number is 38-6005986. These donations can be offset against
your federal and sometimes your state tax return (U.S. tax payers only). For more
information visit the IRS Web-Site, or contact your financial advisor.
Many companies also offer a gift matching program, such that they will match any gift
you make to a non-profit organization. Normally this entails your contacting your human
resources department and sending us a form that the EMU Foundation fills in and returns
to your employer. This is generally a simple administrative procedure that doubles the
value of your gift to LINGUIST, without costing you an extra penny. Please take a moment
to check if your company operates such a program.
Thank you very much for your support of LINGUIST!
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
We would like to thank all of the publishers and subscribers who
donated prizes for this year's Fund Drive games, puzzles and
competitions.
Publisher Prize Donors:
Blackwell
Cascadilla Press
Continuum
Estudios de Sociolingüística
John Benjamins
The Linguistic Association of Finland
Multilingual Matters
MultiLingual magazine
PORTA LINGUARUM
Speculative Grammarian
Springer
The Surrey Morphology Group
Institute of the Czech National Corpus
Subscriber Prize Donors:
Paul G. Chapin
Katherine Chen
Madalena Cruz-Ferreira
Dr. Gabriel Dorta
Suzette Haden Elgin
Gang Gu
Elly van Gelderen
Sara Laviosa
Carol Myers-Scotton
Loraine K. Obler, Ph.D
Ana María Ortega and María Luisa Pérez
Pernille Pennington
Claus Pusch
Michael Swan
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
The LINGUIST List Hall of Fame
ANGELS ($1000 and over)
Arnold Zwicky
MAINSTAYS ($100 to $1000)
A. Henderson, S. Moran, G. Stamm
Andrea Levitt
Andrew Carnie
Anita Fetzer
Antonella Sorace
Arienne M. Dwyer
Aygul Fitton-Brown
Baden Hughes
Barbara H Partee
Bernadine W. Raiskums
Bernard Spolsky
Billy Clark
CaroLyn Green Hartnett
Cecile McKee
Cinzia Russi
Colin Phillips
Columbia School Linguistic Society
Cynthia Edmiston
Cynthia Tia Linn Johnson
D Terence Langendoen
Danny Moates
Daria Suk
David Bowie
Douglas Dee
Dr. MJ Hardman
Earl M. Herrick
Edward Garrett
Elaine J. Francis and Alexander L. Francis
Elizabeth Traugott
Ellen Woolford and John McCarthy
elly van gelderen
Emily M. Bender
Emmon W Bach
Ernest McCarus
Eugenia Casielles
Frances Trix
Geoff Nathan and Margaret Winters
Gerardo Lorenzino
Heike Behrens
Helen Aristar-Dry & Anthony Aristar
Hubert Cuyckens
Jason Boston and Marisa Ferrara
Jeff Good
Jeff Mielke
Jeff Siegel
John Lawler
John Nerbonne
Julie Auger
Justine Cassell
Karen Davis
Karen Milligan
Katy Carlson
Kemp Williams
Keren Rice
Kevin Gregg
Kirk Hazen
Kristy Beers Fägersten
Kübler Sandra
Laura Buszard-Welcher
Laura Callahan
Line Mikkelsen
Manuela Noske
Margaret Speas
Maribel Romero
Mark Overstreet
Mary Shapiro
Michael Hess
Michael Silverstein
Michael Swan
Monica Macaulay and Joe Salmons
Natasha Warner
Neil Olsen
Pacific Languages Unit, USP
Pam and Allen Munro
Paul Chapin
Pius ten Hacken
Prashant Nagaraja
Revue Romane
Rochelle Lieber
Sally McConnell-Ginet
Sandra Fotos
Sanford Steever
Shanley Allen
Shirley Silver
Simin Karimi
Stefan Th. Gries
Steven Gross
University of Pennsylvania Linguistics Club
Ute Römer
Wannie Carstens
Wayles Browne
Wendy Wilkins
Wynn Chao
Wynn Chao and Emmon Bach
Yoko Hasegawa
Zenzi Griffin
- Plus 6 anonymous donors
SUPPORTERS ($50 to $100)
Aaron Huey Sonnenschein
Adam and Andy Ussishkin and Wedel
Alexander Brock
Alice Turk & Bert Remijsen
Alicia Pousada
Alison Gabriele
Andrea C. Schalley
Andrew Linn
Anette Rosenbach
Anja Wanner
Ann Wehmeyer
Anna Fowles-Winkler
Anne Barron
annie zaenen
Aoyama
Arsalan Kahnemuyipour
Barbora Skarabela and Mits Ota
Bonnie D. Schwartz
Bruce Spencer
Carol A. Klee
Cassandre Creswell
Catherine Anderson
Charlotte Brammer
Chih-hsiang Shu
Christina Villafana Dalcher
Christine Gunlogson
Christopher Bader
Claude Mauk
Claus D. Pusch
Clyde Hankey
Cristina Piva
Cynthia Gordon
Deborah Anderson
Don Rubin
Eduardo Urios-Aparisi
Eric Haeberli
Erin O'Rourke
Eva Schultze-Berndt
Florian Jaeger
Francesca Del Gobbo
Francisco Dubert
Frauke Zeller
Gail Stygall
Georgetown University - Graduate Student Linguistics Association
Grover Hudson
Heidi Harley
Henrik Jørgensen
Hernan Emilio Perez
Hortènsia Curell
Huei-ling Lai
Inge Genee
Ingrid Piller
Irina Temnikova
Ivano Caponigro
James Lavine
Jean Mulder
Jennifer Cole
Jennifer L Smith
Jie Zhang
Jila Ghomeshi
Joana Rossello
Joaquim Barbosa
Job M. van Zuijlen
Johanna Laakso
Jose-Luis Mendivil
Josep M. Fontana and Louise McNally
Josep Quer
Joybrato Mukherjee
Juan Carlos Rubio
Judith Meinschaefer
Judy Reilly
kar lok Leung
Karen Corrigan
Kate Paesani
Katherine Appleby
Kathleen M. Ward
Keith Slater
Kevin Burrows
Kristin Denham
Larry LaFond
Laura Downing
Laura McGarrity
Laurie Zaring
Lee Fullerton
Linguistica Occitana (www.revistadoc.org)
Linnaea Stockall
Lisa Davidson
Lisa Galvin
Lise Menn
Ljiljana Progovac
Luis Vicente
M Lynne Murphy
Maite Taboada
Margaret Dunham
Maria Iliescu
Mark Donohue
Mary Swift
Mary Zdrojkowski
Marya Teutsch-Dwyer
Mathias Schulze
Matthias Heinz
Michael Becker
Michael Cahill
Michael Lessard-Clouston
Michael Wagner
Mira Ariel
Nancy Frishberg
Nancy Niedzielski
Nancy Stenson
Nancy Underwood
Naomi Fox
Nicole Dehe
Nobuko Koyama-Murakami
Oliver Stegen
Patricia Cabredo Hofherr
Patricia Donaher
PAUL JUSTICE
Peter Richtsmeier
Pier Marco Bertinetto
Raffaella Folli
Regina Morin
Ricardo etxepare
Rick Nouwen
Robert Englebretson
Robert Hagiwara
Robert Williams
Roberta D'Alessandro
Robin Shoaps
Roderick A. Jacobs
Rodrigo Gutiérrez-Bravo
Roland Pfau
Sarah Fish
Scott Jackson
Scott McGinnis
Seizi Iwata
Shamila Naidoo
Stanley Dubinsky
Student Linguistics Asso, the Ohio State University
Susan Fiksdal
Susan Windisch Brown
Susannah Levi
Susanne Gahl
Sven Grawunder
Taibi NOUR
Theresa Biberauer
Thor Sigurd Nilsen
Tom Roeper
Tom Zurinskas
Trudy Smoke
Veronika Koller
Virginia LoCastro
Wim Vandenbussche
Wolfgang J. Meyer
Yael Sharvit
Yoonjung Kang
- Plus 5 anonymous donors
DONORS (Up to $50)
Adam Buchwald
Adrienne Bruyn
Agnes Sandor
Albert Ortmann
Amina Mettouchi
Andrea Berez
Andrew Koontz-Garboden
Anja Steinlen
Ann Sawyer
Anne Reboul
Anne-Michelle Tessier
Anubha Kothari
Barbara Zurer Pearson
Betty Phillips
Bonny Sands
Brook Danielle Lillehaugen
Catharine Vollmer
Catherine Fortin
Cathryn Donohue
Catie Berkenfield
Chris Sams
Christel de Bruijn
Christopher Becker
David Gaatone
David Goss-Grubbs
David Oshima
Della Chambless
Diana Apoussidou
Dimitrios Ntelitheos
Dipika Mukherjee
Donald F. Reindl
Donna Cromer
Douglas Ball
E. Allyn Smith
Eileen Smith
Elena Battaner-Moro
Elisabeth COTTIER FÁBIÁN
Erik Willis
Fay Wouk
Feride Erku
Galit W. Sassoon
George Williams
Hans Lindquist
Harry Feldman
Hedde Zeijlstra
Heidi Lorimor
Heike Zinsmeister
Helen Stickney
Isabel Perez Jimenez y Norberto Moreno Quiben
Istvan Kecskes
Jacqueline Lecarme
Jacques Jayez
Janet M. Smith
Janice Boynton
Janneke ter Beek
Jason Whitt
Jean-Marc Dewaele
Jenifer Larson-Hall
Jesse Mortelmans
Joanna Lowenstein
John Beavers
Jon Brennan
Jonathan Glenn
Jorge E Porras
Josep Alba
Joshua Viau
Joyce Milambiling
Judith Pine
Judith Tonhauser
Julie Bruch
Karl Reinhardt
Kat Dziwirek
katherine martinez
Katja Jasinskaja
Keir Moulton
Keira Gebbie Ballantyne
Kent Johnson
Kevin Bretonnel Cohen
Koscielecki Marek
Laurie Poulson
Lawrence Rosenwald
Levinson
Lilia Ruiz Debbe
Linda Apse
Lise Dobrin
Lotus Goldberg
Lynsey Wolter
M Victoria Vazquez Rozas
Magnús Snædal
Maher Awad
Mai Kuha
Marek Koscielecki
Margot Rozendaal
Marian Sloboda
Marisol del-Teso-Craviotto
Martin Warin
Mary C. Gruber
Mary Paster
Maurice Wong
Max Wheeler
Mayrene Bentley
Michael Barrie
Michael Maxwell
Michael Pickering
Michelle Fullwood
Miguel Ayerbe
Mike Matloff
Mohammad Haji-Abdolhosseini
Mohammad Jaber
Nancy Melucci
Nancy Stern
Nicholas Fleisher
Olga Gurevich
Pamela Jordan
Peter Slomanson
Pierre Francois Cintas
Rachel Fournier
Raffaella Zanuttini and Bob Frank
Randall Gess
Rebecca T. Cover
Richard Winters
Robert Port
Ron Schaefer
Ronald Schaefer
Sadie Williams
Sandra W. Smith
Sebastian Rasinger
Sergio Baauw
Sherril Condon
Shih-Jen Huang
Shlomo Izre'el
Simona Herdan
Sonya Bird
Stefan Dollinger
Stefan Frisch
Steven Hartman-Keiser
Sumayya Racy
Susan D Fischer
Suzanne Aalberse
Suzanne K. Hilgendorf
Suzette Haden Elgin
Tamina Stephenson
Tania Zamuner
Thera Crane
Theres Grueter
Tomohiro Yanagi
Ute Smit
V J Fedson
Valeria Quochi
Vera Demberg
Vivienne Rogers
Walcir Cardoso
Will Fitzgerald
Winifred Davies
Xose Luis Regueira-Fernandez
Yosuke Sato
Yuri & Mio Backhaus
- Plus 25 anonymous donors
******************************************************
MAJOR SUPPORTING PUBLISHERS
Blackwell Publishing
http://www.blackwellpublishing.com
Cambridge University Press
http://us.cambridge.org
Cascadilla Press
http://www.cascadilla.com/
Continuum International Publishing Group Ltd
http://www.continuumbooks.com
Edinburgh University Press
http://www.eup.ed.ac.uk/
European Language Resources Association
http://www.elda.org/sommaire.php
Georgetown University Press
http://www.press.georgetown.edu
Hodder Arnold
http://www.hoddereducation.co.uk
John Benjamins
http://www.benjamins.com/
http://www.benjamins.nl/
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
http://www.erlbaum.com/
Lincom GmbH
http://www.lincom.at
www.lincom.at
MIT Press
http://mitpress.mit.edu/
Mouton de Gruyter
http://www.mouton-publishers.com
Multilingual Matters
http://www.multilingual-matters.com/
Oxford University Press
http://www.oup.com/us
http://www.oup.co.uk
Palgrave Macmillan
http://www.palgrave.com
Rodopi
http://www.rodopi.nl/
Routledge (Taylor and Francis)
http://www.routledge.com/
Springer
http://www.springeronline.com
OTHER SUPPORTING PUBLISHERS
Anthropological Linguistics
http://www.indiana.edu/~anthling/
CSLI Publications
http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/
Graduate Linguistic Students' Assoc. Umass
http://glsa.hypermart.net/
International Pragmatics Assoc.
http://ipra-www.uia.ac.be/ipra/
Kingston Press Ltd
http://www.kingstonpress.com/
Linguistic Assoc. of Finland
http://www.ling.helsinki.fi/sky/
MIT Working Papers in Linguistics
http://web.mit.edu/mitwpl/
Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics / Landelijke
http://www.lotpublications.nl/
Pacific Linguistics
http://pacling.anu.edu.au/
SIL International
http://www.ethnologue.com/bookstore.asp
St. Jerome Publishing Ltd.
http://www.stjerome.co.uk
Utrecht institute of Linguistics
http://www-uilots.let.uu.nl/
******************************************************
INSTITUTIONS
Aptima, Inc.
Arizona State University
Bilkent University
Birkbeck, University of London
Bucknell University
CACI International Inc.
City University of Hong Kong
Concordia University
DarthDex
Dictaphone
Dublin City University
EML Research gGmbH
European Academy Bozen/Bolzano
European Bioinformatics Institute
European Science Foundation ESF
Franklin Electronic Publishers, Inc.
Gallaudet University
Georgetown University
H5 Technologies
Harvard University Institute of English Language
International Linguistic Association
Janya Inc.
Language Analysis Systems, Inc.
Lund University
McGill University
Michigan State University
Microsoft Corporation
National Security Agency
National Tsing Hua University
North-West University
Northeastern Illinois University
Northwestern University
OFAI - Austrian Research Inst. for AI
Priberam Informática
Rozetta
Simon Fraser University
Stanford University
SVOX AG
Swarthmore College
SYSTRAN Software Inc.
Szanca Solutions, Inc.
Thomson Legal & Regulatory
Tufts University
UCLA
Universitaet Konstanz
Universitaet Leipzig
University of Alberta
University of British Columbia
University of Calgary
University of Cambridge
University of Chicago
University of Cincinnati
University of Cyprus
University of Edinburgh
University of Florida
University of Fribourg, Suisse
University of Geneva - ETI
University of Goettingen
University of Hamburg
University of Heidelberg
University of Helsinki
University of Illinois
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign (UIUC)
University of Konstanz
University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban
University of Leipzig
University of Maryland
University of Maryland, College Park
University of Melbourne
University of Michigan
University of Oregon
University of Oslo
University of Pittsburgh
University of Potsdam
University of Reading
University of Rochester
University of Southampton
University of Southern Denmark
University of Stuttgart
University of Texas at Austin
University of Victoria
Universität Tübingen
Université de Neuchâtel
Université du Québec à Montréal
Voice Signal Technologies
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam
-----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-17-1055
More information about the LINGUIST
mailing list