21.2575, Review: Linguistic Theories; Phonology: Dresher (2009)
linguist at LINGUISTLIST.ORG
linguist at LINGUISTLIST.ORG
Sat Jun 12 11:24:18 UTC 2010
LINGUIST List: Vol-21-2575. Sat Jun 12 2010. ISSN: 1068 - 4875.
Subject: 21.2575, Review: Linguistic Theories; Phonology: Dresher (2009)
Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Eastern Michigan U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>
Reviews: Monica Macaulay, U of Wisconsin-Madison
Eric Raimy, U of Wisconsin-Madison
Joseph Salmons, U of Wisconsin-Madison
Anja Wanner, U of Wisconsin-Madison
<reviews at linguistlist.org>
Homepage: http://linguistlist.org/
The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University,
and donations from subscribers and publishers.
Editor for this issue: Joseph Salmons <jsalmons at linguistlist.org>
================================================================
This LINGUIST List issue is a review of a book published by one of our
supporting publishers, commissioned by our book review editorial staff. We
welcome discussion of this book review on the list, and particularly invite
the author(s) or editor(s) of this book to join in. If you are interested in
reviewing a book for LINGUIST, look for the most recent posting with the subject
"Reviews: AVAILABLE FOR REVIEW", and follow the instructions at the top of the
message. You can also contact the book review staff directly.
===========================Directory==============================
1)
Date: 11-Jun-2010
From: Grover Hudson < hudson at msu.edu >
Subject: The Contrastive Hierarchy in Phonology
-------------------------Message 1 ----------------------------------
Date: Sat, 12 Jun 2010 07:20:44
From: Grover Hudson [hudson at msu.edu]
Subject: The Contrastive Hierarchy in Phonology
E-mail this message to a friend:
http://linguistlist.org/issues/emailmessage/verification.cfm?iss=21-2575.html&submissionid=2637894&topicid=9&msgnumber=1
Discuss this message:
http://linguistlist.org/pubs/reviews/get-review.cfm?subid=2637894
Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/20/20-3272.html
AUTHOR: B. Elan Dresher
TITLE: The Contrastive Hierarchy in Phonology
SERIES TITLE: Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 121
PUBLISHER: Cambridge University Press
YEAR: 2009
Grover Hudson, Program in Linguistics, Michigan State University
SUMMARY
According to the publisher's announcement of this book (Linguist List 20.3272)
contrast 'is one of the most central concepts in linguistics'. The book
'presents an original account of the logic and history of contrast in
phonology', 'provides empirical evidence from diverse phonological domains that
only contrastive features are computed by the phonological component of
grammar', and 'argues that the contrastive specifications of phonemes are
governed by language-particular feature hierarchies'. It assigns 'a key role to
abstract cognitive structures' (the feature hierarchies), and challenges
'contemporary approaches that favor phonetic explanations of phonological
phenomena'. There are nine chapters as follows.
Ch. 1, 'Introduction'. Rather than 'phonetic contrast' as in phonemic theory,
contrast here is 'pattern alignment' somewhat in the sense of Sapir 1925, what D
(2; bare numbers in parentheses are pages of the book) terms 'the arrangement of
the phonemes of a language, their place in the phonological system'. D reviews
some highlights of the theory of contrast in phonology, with the conclusion that
modern phonology should 'reconnect with its roots' and 'establish phonological
contrast as a central principle of phonological theory' (8). Finally, chapter 1
presents the plan of the book by chapters.
Ch. 2, 'The logic of contrast'. There is a connection to phonemic analysis in
the generative grammar sense: how the minimal feature specifications of phonemes
(phonemic contrast by phonological features) are discovered. D's idea is that
this should be done by 'the Successive Division Algorithm' (16), not by 'the
Pairwise Algorithm' (14). Here it must suffice to say that the former involves
sorting the phonemes by features in a succession of top-down splits such that
feature content may differ depending on the ordering. For example, given /p, b,
m/, sorting these first by [voiced] then [nasal] leaves /p/ unspecified for
nasality, because /p/ was fully distinct as [-voiced]. Sorting first by [nasal]
leaves /m/ unspecified for voice, because /m/ was fully distinct as [+nasal].
D's idea is that one or the other ordering will be preferred according to
whether [voiced] or [nasal] is more 'active' in the phonology. The rest of the
chapter is concerned to show that the pairwise method, making every phoneme pair
contrast by at least one feature, doesn't work anyway.
Ch. 3, 'Contrast in structuralist phonology'. Work of Sapir, Trubetzkoy,
Martinet, Jakobson and Lotz, and Hockett is surveyed to show that 'issues of
phonological contrast were central to their thinking', despite 'absence of an
explicit or consistent approach to assigning contrastive features' (71).
According to Trubetzkoy (1969: 100), for example, Artshi consonant rounding is
neutralized next to /u, o/, so these 'are placed in opposition with the
remaining vowels' /i, e, a/, and must be specified as [+round] rather than
[+back, -low], as seems otherwise possible and as in Latin (56-57). If /u, o/
are specified [+back, -low], then [round] is redundant and non-contrastive in
them. D concludes from such examples the 'Contrastivist Hypothesis' (74: of Hall
2007: 20): 'The phonological component of a language L operates only on those
features which are necessary to distinguish the phonemes of L from one another',
such as [round] in Artshi. Such 'active' features are ordered above others (are
higher) in the 'contrastive hierarchy', which renders the others less
contrastive / more redundant. 'Active' characteristics of contrastive
(non-redundant) features include a role in conditioning or undergoing
alternations and neutralizations, co-occurrence relations, and the adaptation of
sounds of other languages (72).
Ch. 4, 'The rise and fall of the contrastive hierarchy'. Jakobson's collaborator
Halle, (especially Halle 1959) promoted a contrastive hierarchy significantly
inspired by Jakobson's theory of ordered phonological-feature acquisition by
children (Jakobson 1941), implemented in grammars as a tree diagram in which
higher(-ranked) features are acquired earlier, for example [+vocalic] and
[+nasal] in Russian, which tend to make lower-ranked [voiced] unspecified (91).
Stanley's (1967) influential critique of Halle's approach showed how the
unspecified (zero-)values could function as additional, third, values in what
had been agreed to be a binary-feature system. D agrees (98), but finds this
'inconsistent with the Contrastivist Hypothesis', in which 'the phonology treats
contrastive specifications differently from noncontrastive specifications'; that
is, zeroes do function as third values. But while Halle's system assigned
zero-voiced to the Russian coronal affricates and /x/, which lack contrastive
voiced parallels, in D's system as triggers of voiceless assimilation in
obstruent sequences these must be assigned 'active' [-voiced] (95).
Ch. 5, 'Generative phonology: contrast goes underground'. Chomsky and Halle
(1968) discontinued Halle's feature hierarchies in favor of a universal but
incomplete theory of context-sensitive feature markedness. As D (108) says: 'no
well-formed branching tree can model these markedness conventions'. In the
1980s, theories of phonological 'activity' based on contrastiveness reappeared:
in Kiparsky's (1985) Lexical Phonology, and the opposed 'underspecification'
theories of Archangeli (1984) and Steriade (1995). Non-contrastive features are
inert in Kiparsky's lexical stratum (e.g. typically voice in sonorants), but
become active in the post-lexical (allophonic) stratum (where sonorants can
assimilate voice). In 'Radical Underspecification' (Archangeli), only the marked
value of a feature is lexical, and not even this if it is derivable, like
[+voiced] typically in sonorants, but also all -F if +F is lexical. Feature
markedness depends on language-specific feature activity, prominently evidenced
by the minimally specified, inactive, vowel. In 'Contrastive Underspecification'
(Steriade), both values of a contrastive feature are lexical, e.g. typically
[voiced] in obstruents. Non-contrastive (redundant) features are zeroed, e.g.
[-high] in /a/ given contrastive [+low]. Unspecified for [high], /a/ may be
neutral toward height harmony, but is zeroed as logically redundant (127). In
'Feature Geometry' (cf. Clements and Hume 1995), feature hierarchies are
implemented in feature trees in which, in assimilations, specified features
spread to nodes unspecified for the features.
Ch. 6, 'Contrast in Optimality Theory'. D surveys and exemplifies Optimality
Theory (OT) constraints which optimize language-specific feature hierarchies (in
which noncontrastive specifications are absent). These are faithfulness
constraints preserving particular features e.g. Max [sonorant], and markedness
constraints which disprefer feature combinations e.g. *[round, +low]). Lacking
in OT is a constraint type or theory of constraints which might optimize D's
theory (161) that the Contrastivist Hypothesis is 'implemented by the Successive
Division Algorithm operating on a contrastive feature hierarchy', and 'the
phonological component of a language L operates only on' the contrastive and
specified features established by the hierarchy.
Ch. 7, 'Evidence for the contrastive hierarchy in phonology'. Five sorts of data
for D's theory are examined, concerning vowel harmony, metaphony (regressive
vowel-to-vowel assimilation), consonant co-occurrence restrictions, loanword
adaptation, and child acquisition. An example of each is mentioned here.
Classical Manchu vowel harmony is said to reflect its contrastive vowel
hierarchy according to which vowels participate unexpectedly in assimilations:
for example open-/o/ triggers labial harmony but /u/, being contrastively back
and unspecified for labial, doesn't; schwa triggers [+ATR] harmony (against
[-ATR] /a/) but ATR-noncontrastive /i/ doesn't (177). Pasiego Montañes metaphony
is an argument for different contrastive hierarchies in different 'domains'
within a language -- here, stem vs. affix vowels (192). Bumo Izon consonant
co-occurrence restricts words to either plosive or implosive, except for the
plosive and implosive velars, which co-occur with both sets, a result D explains
by the implosive velar being labio-velar so arguably unspecified for the
implosion (glottalic) feature. Hawaiian and Maori adapt English coronal
obstruents differently (Hawaiian as /k/ and Maori as /h/), explained by absence
of glottal stop in the latter, with the result that /k/ is the default obstruent
in Hawaiian and /h/ in Maori (201). Since Jakobson (1941), phoneme acquisition
order has been explained by a feature hierarchy, famously unmarked [a,i] before
[u]. Concerning acquisition, against Pinker's (1994: 265) claim that learners
have phonemic behavior 'before they produce or understand words', D (205) finds
it 'hard to see how infants can acquire phonemes without knowing if two
utterances are same or different' (contrast).
Ch. 8, 'Other approaches to contrast in phonology'. Several 'other approaches',
all 'advanced in the recent phonological literature', are found wanting to the
extent they diverge from D's theory. Included are Dispersion Theory (Padgett
2003), Structured Specification (Frisch, Pierrehumbert and Broe 2004), Clements'
(2009) argument for universally determined feature markedness, and appeals to
'minimal contrast ... in the work of a number of contemporary phonologists',
which 'arises from the concept of pairwise comparison, which was shown to have
fatal flaws in chapter 2'.
Ch. 9, 'Conclusion'. This 2-page chapter reviews main points to conclude that
'the contrastive hierarchy' is a 'pivotal principle of linguistic structure',
and 'the Contrastivist Hypothesis is a fruitful approach to phonological theory'.
EVALUATION
Especially given D's thorough demonstration of the long history in phonology of
deferring the matter, it does seem important, now, to seek principles of
contrast in phonological feature analysis. Agreement on the evidence, however,
and even on the data, seems sure to prove difficult in the absence of agreement
on the feature set, and with D's acceptance of 'abstract' analyses employing
unphonetic lexical form. D accepts and employs privative and binary features,
some of both, and actually ternary features since he admits zero as a third
value in addition to plus and minus; he uses both ATR and RTR; front, coronal,
and back; peripheral, vibrant, etc. Schwa is termed non-low in Inuit (165) and
low in Classical Manchu (176). Concerning Inupiaq (165), Nez Perce (185), and
Yawlumne (=Yawelmani, 208), critical data involve (underlying) contrasts never
heard in the languages ('absolute neutralization').
The theory seems to require every language to offer learners decisive evidence
for its contrastive hierarchy: phonological 'activity' evidenced consistently if
variously (72), contra Hockett (1955: 174, quoted p. 57): 'a given feature or
difference turns up in some contexts as of primary relevance, in other contexts
as subsidiary'. Regarding the problem of 'abstractness', the Inuit/Inupiaq data
(164-7) may be instructive. There are dialects having 'strong' and 'weak' /i/.
Strong /i/ 'palatalizes' alveolars and weak /i/ doesn't; the latter, said to be
underlying historical schwa, also 'undergoes a variety of assimilation and
deletion processes'. Most dialects, however, no longer distinguish 'the two
kinds of /i/', and in these /i/ does not palatalize. D's interpretation is that
in the latter dialects /u/ is labial and /a/ is low, so /i/ necessarily lacks
'some contrastive feature that triggers palatalization' (165, footnote).
There is another interpretation: in the palatalizing dialects instead of
underlying schwa there is in addition to ordinary /i/ an exceptional,
palatalizing, and lexically encoded /i/ (encoded in the manner of /f/s in
English which alternate with /v/s as in life/lives). This is indeed the marked
vowel, so the alternation has naturally been levelled in the majority dialects
in favor of unmarked (probably more frequent) ordinary /i/. Necessarily the
result is no palatalization. D's analysis works, but cannot explain why the
palatalization-alternation was levelled, except by invoking some principle
different from that (preference for one-form, one-meaning) which elsewhere and
ordinarily explains levelling (as of the English f~v alternation). (Then there
is the problem, mentioned above, of the palatalizing feature -- presumably not
[coronal], which is said to be contrastive for 'active' /i/.)
D argues persuasively from a variety of data and with insightful reference to a
long history and a breadth of theoretical perspectives. Helpful (if in a longer
book) would be data from a single language across the range of phenomena
thought to provide consistent evidence of phonological 'activity' supporting a
contrastive hierarchy. The most exemplified language of the book, Classical
Manchu, unfortunately lacks speakers and acquirers who could provide such data.
(For analyses of this language differing from that presented by D, see Ard 1984.)
REFERENCES
Archangeli, Diana (1984). Underspecification in Yawelmani phonology and
morphology. PhD dissertation, Cambridge: MIT (published 1988, New York: Garland
Press).
Ard, Josh (1984). Vowel harmony in Manchu: a critical overview. Journal of
Linguistics 20.57-80.
Chomsky, Noam, and Morris Halle (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York:
Harper & Row.
Clements, G. N. (2009). The role of features in phonological inventories,
Contemporary views on architecture and representations in phonological theory,
Eric Raimy and Charles Cairns, eds., 19-68. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Clements, G. N, and Elizabeth Hume (1995). The internal organization of speech
sounds, Handbook of phonological theory, John Goldsmith, ed., 245-306. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Frisch, Stephen, Janet Pierrehumbert, and Michael Broe (2004). Similarity
avoidance and the OCP. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 22.179-228.
Hall, Daniel C. (2007). The role and representation of contrast in phonological
theory, PhD dissertation, Toronto: University of Toronto.
Halle, Morris (1959). The sound pattern of Russian. The sound pattern of
Russian: a statistical and acoustic investigation. The Hague: Mouton.
Hockett, Charles (1955). A manual of phonology (Indiana University Publications
in Linguistics 11). Baltimore: Waverly Press.
Jakobson, Roman (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze.
Uppsala: Uppsala Universitets Arsskrift (translated by A. R. Keilor, 1968. Child
Language, Aphasia, and Linguistic Universals. The Hague: Mouton).
Kiparsky, Paul. 1985. Some consequences of Lexical Phonology, Phonology Yearbook
2: 85-138.
Padgett, Jaye (2003). Contrast and post-velar fronting in Russian. Natural
Language and Linguistic Theory 21.39-87.
Pinker, Steven (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow.
Sapir, Edward (1925). Sound patterns in language. Language 1.37-51 (also 1957,
Readings in Linguistics I, Martin Joos, ed., 19-25. Washington: Council of
Learned Societies).
Stanley, Richard (1967). Redundancy rules in phonology, Language 43.393-436.
Steriade, Donca (1995). Underspecification and markedness, Handbook of
phonological theory, John Goldsmith, ed., 114-174. Oxford: Blackwell.
Trubetzkoy, N. S. (1939). Grundzüge der Phonologie. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck &
Ruprecht (translated by Christiane Baltaxe, 1969. Principles of phonology.
Berkeley: University of California Press).
Underhill, Robert (1976). The case for an abstract segment in Greenlandic.
International Journal of American Linguistics 42.349-358.
ABOUT THE REVIEWER
Grover Hudson taught phonology, historical linguistics, and Ethiopian
linguistics including Amharic language at Michigan State University. He is
author of a comparative dictionary of Highland East Cushitic languages, an
introductory linguistics textbook, with Anbessa Teferra a recent book on
Amharic, and articles on phonology and Ethiopian descriptive and historical
linguistics.
-----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-21-2575
More information about the LINGUIST
mailing list