27.2134, Review: Historical Ling; Ling Theories; Semantics; Syntax: Gildea, Sommerer, Barðdal, Smirnova (2015)

The LINGUIST List via LINGUIST linguist at listserv.linguistlist.org
Mon May 9 17:49:04 UTC 2016


LINGUIST List: Vol-27-2134. Mon May 09 2016. ISSN: 1069 - 4875.

Subject: 27.2134, Review: Historical Ling; Ling Theories; Semantics; Syntax: Gildea, Sommerer, Barðdal, Smirnova (2015)

Moderators: linguist at linguistlist.org (Damir Cavar, Malgorzata E. Cavar)
Reviews: reviews at linguistlist.org (Anthony Aristar, Helen Aristar-Dry, Robert Coté, Sara Couture)
Homepage: http://linguistlist.org

*****************    LINGUIST List Support    *****************
                       Fund Drive 2016
                   25 years of LINGUIST List!
Please support the LL editors and operation with a donation at:
           http://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/

Editor for this issue: Sara  Couture <sara at linguistlist.org>
================================================================


Date: Mon, 09 May 2016 13:48:36
From: David Lorenz [david.lorenz at anglistik.uni-freiburg.de]
Subject: Diachronic Construction Grammar

 
Discuss this message:
http://linguistlist.org/pubs/reviews/get-review.cfm?subid=36123937


Book announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/26/26-3587.html

EDITOR: Jóhanna  Barðdal
EDITOR: Elena  Smirnova
EDITOR: Lotte  Sommerer
EDITOR: Spike  Gildea
TITLE: Diachronic Construction Grammar
SERIES TITLE: Constructional Approaches to Language 18
PUBLISHER: John Benjamins
YEAR: 2015

REVIEWER: David Lorenz, Universität Freiburg

Reviews Editor: Robert Arthur Cote

SUMMARY

Diachronic studies in Construction Grammar have received increasing attention
in the past decade. Notable larger publications include an edited volume
(Bergs & Diewald, eds. 2008), a special issue of “Cognitive Linguistics”
(Hoffmann & Trousdale, eds. 2011) and two book-length treatments (Hilpert
2013, Traugott & Trousdale 2013). Now “Diachronic Construction Grammar”,
edited by Jóhanna Barðdal, Elena Smirnova, Lotte Sommerer and Spike Gildea
adds to this body of research and presents studies that address relevant
issues in grammatical change from a constructional perspective.

The volume comprises seven articles and a foreword by Willem Hollmann
dedicated to the memory of Anna Siewierska. (The first, and longest,
contribution is an overview article by Barðdal & Gildea. The other six are
research articles on various processes and instances of language change taking
a Construction Grammar perspective. Some present theoretical considerations
and support them with case studies (Traugott, Smirnova); others present case
studies from which theoretical hypotheses are derived (Sommerer, Fried,
Torrent, Colleman). They cover a range of different languages: German
(Smirnova), (Old) English (Traugott, Sommerer), (Old) Czech (Fried), Brazilian
Portuguese (Torrent), and Dutch (Colleman).

The editors take an inclusive approach to Construction Grammar and do not
commit to a specific strand of the theory; thus, the common denominator in the
articles is the set of basic assumptions of Construction Grammar listed in
Barðdal & Gildea’s opening article (p.10f):
Constructions are pairings of form and meaning, and as such, they are the
basic building blocks of language
There is uniform representation of grammatical structures in that all
linguistic units are viewed as form-meaning pairings
Constructions are organized in taxonomic dichotomies or hierarchies
The theory is monostratal, with no surface structure - D-structure distinction
There is no distinction between ‘core’ and ‘periphery’

The individual contributions are summarized in the following.

In their introductory article, “Diachronic Construction Grammar:
Epistemological context, basic assumptions and historical implications”,
Barðdal & Gildea “discuss the value of the Construction Grammar framework to
solving perceived problems with diachronic syntax” (1). They start out from
the “clear contrast between the (plausible) reconstruction of historical
phonology and the (implausible) reconstruction of historical morphosyntax” (3)
assumed by both Neogrammarians and Generative Grammar. Diachronic morphosyntax
came into focus with grammaticalization research (Barðdal & Gildea avoid the
term ‘grammaticalization theory’), which has accumulated evidence of
systematic mechanisms in morphosyntactic change. Thus grammaticalization
research is seen as the main forerunner of diachronic construction grammar;
references to grammaticalization are frequent not only in this article but
throughout the book (especially in Traugott’s and Smirnova’s contributions,
see below). Barðdal & Gildea specifically note a number of studies from the
1990s that consider grammaticalization in a larger syntactic context, and
which already show “the broad strokes of diachronic construction grammar” (8),
albeit without a well-defined notion of ‘construction’; they specifically
point to Harris & Campbell’s (1995) model of syntactic change based on three
mechanisms: Reanalysis, Extension and Contact. 

The heart of the article is an outline of morphosyntactic change from a
construction grammar perspective, which comes under the modest heading “The
basics of construction grammar and its diachronic implications” (10; ch.3).
This section also incorporates summaries of the other papers in the volume,
not as a simple listing but each at the relevant point of the discussion. In
its basic assumptions, the outline follows prior work on the topic (most
notably Traugott & Trousdale 2013), e.g. in the distinction of
constructionalization –the emergence of a new construction– and constructional
change (changes within an existing construction), as well as in positing three
basic steps in constructionalization:

a semantically new use of a construction (sem1 -> sem2);
a syntactic reanalysis (syn1 -> syn2);
the new, distinct construction (sem2-syn2) undergoes further changes
(‘actualization’).

Further basics of construction grammar with diachronic implications are the
mental network of construction (the ConstructiCon) and the relative
positioning of constructions on clines, e.g. from lexical to schematic. Here,
the central questions, diachronic or otherwise, refer to how related
constructions affect each other, such as whether a construction always
‘inherits’ the features of the more schematic construction it instantiates.

In the sections on usage-based approaches and the productivity of
constructions, Barðdal & Gildea discuss the role of type and token
frequencies. Entrenchment of a schema is strengthened by high type frequency,
while token frequency strengthens the specific instances. Productivity is
linked to type frequency, but token frequency can also contribute to it
through analogy. Moreover, a construction of low type frequency may be
productive if it has high ‘coherence’ (the degree of internal consistency
between the types) and low ‘schematicity’ (the area it covers within a given
functional-semantic space). Coherence and schematicity are the key elements of
Barðdal & Gildea’s model of constructional competition, where an existing
construction A faces competition from an incoming construction B. B initially
has a low type frequency, but slowly expands while retaining its coherence;
with B encroaching, A loses types, but in a way that retains its schematicity,
not its coherence. Thus at a stage when A and B are even in terms of type
frequency, B has higher coherence and is better poised to take over the
functional domain they compete for.

Elizabeth Traugott’s contribution, “Toward a coherent account of grammatical
constructionalization”, sets out to revise issues of grammaticalization in
constructional terms. Traugott proposes a concept of grammatical
constructionalization, which describes the development of procedural
constructions (i.e. grammatical items). The distinction between contentful
(lexical) and procedural (grammatical) meanings is gradient, and change is
gradual and proceeds by a sequence of small steps, labeled as Innovation,
Conventionalization, Constructionalization and Post-constructionalization.
Traugott argues that this view of grammatical constructionalization as a
“mixture of meaning and form changes” (59) is advantageous because, among
other things, it does not segregate form from function but addresses the link
between them, and it captures not only change in simple, atomic items, but
also in more complex, schematic ones. 

A special focus is on the roles of reanalysis and analogy in grammatical
constructionalization. Here, Traugott argues for a distinction between the
cognitive processes which enable change and the observable mechanisms of
change. She suggests the terms parsing (cognitive motivation) and neoanalysis
(mechanism) for reanalysis, and analogical thinking (cognitive motivation) and
analogization (mechanism) for analogy. In a constructional view, analogical
thinking is a crucial factor in language change; as for the mechanisms,
neoanalysis is more central, since it is involved in all change, including
analogization. These considerations are then exemplified in a revisit of a
textbook example of grammaticalization, the development of the ‘going to’
future.

In the next article, “Constructionalization and constructional change: The
role of context in the development of constructions”, Elena Smirnova presents
a model of constructionalization that focuses on the role of context, based on
similar models for grammaticalization (e.g. Diewald & Smirnova 2012). It
consists of three stages: Untypical contexts, where an innovative use of a
construction sets the preconditions for further change; Critical contexts, in
which the construction is semantically and structurally ambiguous between an
“old” and a “new” reading; and Isolating contexts which only allow the “new”
interpretation, thus consolidating grammaticalization. In terms of
constructionalization, this means that critical contexts instantiate both an
existing and a new construction. The main characteristic of
constructionalization then is the development of such critical contexts. Thus,
Smirnova defines constructionalization as “the formation of a new construction
by way of gradual accumulation and strengthening of contextual restrictions
with resulting semantic and structural reorganization of language material”
(89). This involves the semanticization of pragmatic implicatures in untypical
contexts and structural reanalysis in critical contexts. Smirnova exemplifies
this model by three case studies from German: «scheinen + zu» -infinitive,
«würde» + infinitive and «gehören».

In the following articles, “The influence of constructions in
grammaticalization: Revisiting category emergence and the development of the
definite article in English”, Lotte Sommerer analyses the grammaticalization
of the Old English demonstrative «se» into the definite article ‘the’ as a
case of grammatical constructionalization. The central mechanisms of this are
analogy and frequency. Sommerer sees the advantage of a constructional
approach in the focus on larger constructions rather than atomic items. Thus,
the object of her analysis is not the demonstrative/determiner but the entire
Noun Phrase in Old English. Here, Sommerer observes three preferences:
although definiteness marking is optional, the noun is typically preceded by
at least one determinative (possessive pronoun, genitive construction or
demonstrative); the element determining reference is leftmost; the
demonstrative is by far the most frequent determinative. This triggers a
reanalysis to a Noun Phrase with an obligatory determinative slot at the left
and the demonstrative as the default filler of that slot. This analysis is
strengthened by taxonomic influence, as the most frequent structure of Old
English Noun Phrases (definite or indefinite) have one item preceding the
noun. Sommerer suggests that this preference is then applied schematically to
less abstract levels (i.e. the definite NP). The result is
constructionalization, the emergence of a new construction Det+N.

Mirjam Fried’s article “Irregular morphology in regular syntactic patterns: A
case of constructional re-alignment” deals with the formation of participle
adjectives (PAs) in Old Czech. Next to the regular formation pattern
(V-«ú-c-», e.g. «kajúcí» repenting) there is a set of irregular forms which
are morphologically intransparent (e.g. «min-ujúc»- passing), which Fried
calls ‘pseudo-PAs’. PAs and pseudo-PAs show different tendencies in function
and in the syntactic structures they occur in. Semantically, pseudo-PAs show
fewer traces of their verbal origin than regular PAs; both occur in
constructions that are either more compatible with regular PAs or with
pseudo-PAs. The crucial observation then is that when there is a ‘mismatch’
between item and construction, e.g. a pseudo-PA in construction more
compatible with regular PAs, the pseudo-PA often compensates for this by
displaying more features of (action) verbs. Diachronically, this leads to
mutual adjustments in the word forms and the constructions. Their mutual
influence eventually leads to a fixation of categorial status, i.e. the
pseudo-PAs becoming full-fledged adjectives. This may be considered a case of
constructionalization, though not in the sense that there be a new
construction where before there was none, but in terms of a new “conventional
affiliation between an item and a syntactic pattern of a certain type” (168). 

Tiago Timponi Torrent’s article is entitled “On the relation between
inheritance and change: The Constructional Convergence and the Construction
Network Reconfiguration Hypothesis”. The Constructional Convergence Hypothesis
states that “historically unrelated constructions are capable of participating
in the same formally and functionally motivated network through a series of
changes that cause their form and meaning to merge into an already existing
pattern” (175); the Construction Network Reconfiguration Hypothesis that
“inheritance relations in construction networks change over time as new
constructions emerge” (ibid.). These are exemplified by the development of the
Para Infinitive family of constructions in Brazilian Portuguese. The Para
Infinitive pattern emerges in the 13th century out of the convergence of four
constructions which have diverse origins but come to be associated with one
another due to developing formal and semantic similarities (Constructional
Convergence). The family is extended further in subsequent centuries through
various processes of grammaticalization, analogy and chunking. In the process,
the existing relations between constructions in the family undergo changes,
which can lead to new synchronic inheritance links between historically
unrelated constructions (Construction Network Reconfiguration).

In “Constructionalization and post-constructionalization: The constructional
semantics of the Dutch «krijgen»-passive from a diachronic perspective”,
Timothy Colleman investigates the rather recent innovation of passives with
the verb «krijgen» ‘get’ in Dutch. From its earliest attestations around 1900,
this construction is strikingly productive; Colleman identifies four semantic
clusters in data from the early twentieth century, suggesting that the
«krijgen»-passive has developed from multiple sources in parallel, through
“not one but several critical constructions” (234). In its further development
the construction shows various semantic extensions (e.g. to projected rather
than actual transfer, and to verbs of communication), which are seen as a
post-constructionalization loss of restrictions. As a result the
«krijgen»-passive now has a different semantic range than the corresponding
active double-object construction, which shows its independence as a
construction within “a family of ‘transfer’ constructions” (247).

EVALUATION

The book is excellent in what it is, and if it has its limitations, they are
few and rather lie in what it is not. If we consider the current popularity of
Construction Grammar, and that virtually everything in language has a
diachronic dimension, then “Diachronic Construction Grammar” is a very wide
field. A collective volume like the book reviewed here cannot achieve a
complete coverage of everything that is, or might be, diachronic construction
grammar. For example, probabilistic variationist approaches are lacking from
the collection (but constructional competition is treated in the overview
article). That said, the volume achieves a coherent collection of excellent
and well-presented studies, opened by a comprehensive and inspired overview
article. The overview article draws on the  contributions as well as many
findings from the last decades, which makes it a very worthwhile read. My only
and very minor complaint is that the specific cases are often presented
without examples and remain abstract.

Another accomplishment of this book is that each of the contributions works as
a cogent study of a specific phenomenon but with a clear aim of contributing
to the larger picture of a diachronic construction grammar. The overview
article incorporates all the contributions in its outline of morphosyntactic
change, and many of the articles also make references to other contributions
to the volume. This gives the book a strong coherence and invites the reader
to consider the connections and differences between the studies.

The book relates to other recent work in diachronic construction grammar and
makes some relevant contributions to current topics in this field. I would
like to specifically draw attention to two issues here: the notions of
constructional change and constructionalization, and the network relations
between constructions.

In their review of Traugott & Trousdale’s 2013 book, Börjars et al. (2015)
raise doubts about the usefulness of the concepts of constructionalization and
constructional change, arguing that it is not clear “what it means to be a new
construction as opposed to being the same construction, but changed” (374).
There may be no ultimate answer to this question, but the papers in
“Diachronic Construction Grammar” address the issue with some useful insights.
Constructionalization is described as a process of small steps (Traugott) and
gradual reorganization (Smirnova, Torrent) that is propelled by innovations
and preferences in usage (Sommerer, Fried, Colleman). These are not completely
new observations, but their importance is integral to a constructional
approach to change; constructions as generalizations over concrete utterances
will change gradually with shifts in usage frequencies and contexts of use. 

Moreover, constructions are connected in a network; thus, what changes is not
just individual constructions, but also constructions’ relations to each
other. This is mostly spelled out in terms of taxonomic relations, i.e.
instantiation and inheritance links. For example, an extending construction
comes to instantiate more types (e.g. Colleman, Smirnova), or some lower-level
constructions’ affiliations with more schematic constructions may change (e.g.
Fried, Torrent). Less prominent in the studies in this volume are associative
relations between constructions on the same level of abstraction (horizontal
links, which include subpart links and associations evidenced through priming
effects, cf. Diessel 2015). As these are part of the constructional network,
they surely have a role in constructional change as well. To spell this out is
a matter for future research; yet some of the concepts presented in this
volume can be seen as pointing in this direction, specifically analogical
thinking (Traugott), the role of context (Smirnova) and network
reconfiguration (Torrent).

In sum, the editors and authors have produced a very readable and stimulating
volume that will certainly play an important role in the further development
of diachronic construction grammar. With its aim of gaining a better
understanding of change by taking a constructionist stance, the focus is not
too narrow, nor too wide. As a result, the book will be relevant to any
scholar interested in morphosyntactic change, as well as to construction
grammarians interested in the diachronic dimension of grammar.

REFERENCES

Bergs, Alexander & Gabriele Diewald (eds.). 2008. Constructions and language
change. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Börjars, Kersti, Nigel Vincent & George Walkden. 2015. On constructing a
theory of grammatical change. Transactions of the Philological Society 113(3).
363-382.

Diessel, Holger. 2015. Usage-based construction grammar. In Ewa Dąbrowska &
Dagmar Divjak (eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de
Gruyter. 296-322.

Diewald, Gabriele & Elena Smirnova. 2012. Paradigmatic integration: The fourth
stage in an expanded grammaticalization scenario. In Kristin Davidse, Tine
Breban, Lieselotte Brems & Tanja Mortelmans (eds.), Grammaticalization and
language change: New reflections, 111-134. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Harris, Alice C. & Lyle Campbell. 1995. Historical syntax in cross-linguistic
perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hilpert, Martin. 2013. Constructional change in English. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.

Hoffmann, Thomas & Graeme Trousdale (eds.). 2011. Variation, change and
constructions in English. Special Issue of Cognitive Linguistics 22(1).

Traugott, Elizabeth C. & Graeme Trousdale. 2013. Constructionalization and
constructional change. Oxford: Oxford University Press.


ABOUT THE REVIEWER

David Lorenz is a post-doctoral researcher and lecturer in English Linguistics
at Albert-Ludwigs-Universität Freiburg. His current research interests are
cognitive linguistics, construction grammar and language variation & change.





------------------------------------------------------------------------------

*****************    LINGUIST List Support    *****************
                       Fund Drive 2016
Please support the LL editors and operation with a donation at:
            http://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/

This year the LINGUIST List hopes to raise $79,000. This money 
will go to help keep the List running by supporting all of our 
Student Editors for the coming year.

Don't forget to check out Fund Drive 2016 site!

http://funddrive.linguistlist.org/

For all information on donating, including information on how to 
donate by check, money order, PayPal or wire transfer, please visit:
http://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/

The LINGUIST List is under the umbrella of Indiana University and
as such can receive donations through Indiana University Foundation. We
also collect donations via eLinguistics Foundation, a registered 501(c)
Non Profit organization with the federal tax number 45-4211155. Either
way, the donations can be offset against your federal and sometimes your
state tax return (U.S. tax payers only). For more information visit the
IRS Web-Site, or contact your financial advisor.

Many companies also offer a gift matching program, such that
they will match any gift you make to a non-profit organization.
Normally this entails your contacting your human resources department
and sending us a form that the Indiana University Foundation fills in
and returns to your employer. This is generally a simple administrative
procedure that doubles the value of your gift to LINGUIST, without
costing you an extra penny. Please take a moment to check if
your company operates such a program.


Thank you very much for your support of LINGUIST!
 


----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-27-2134	
----------------------------------------------------------
Visit LL's Multitree project for over 1000 trees dynamically generated
from scholarly hypotheses about language relationships:
          http://multitree.org/








More information about the LINGUIST mailing list