32.1901, Review: Anthropological Linguistics; General Linguistics; Sociolinguistics: Güldemann, McConvell, Rhodes (2020)
The LINGUIST List
linguist at listserv.linguistlist.org
Wed Jun 2 03:51:39 UTC 2021
LINGUIST List: Vol-32-1901. Tue Jun 01 2021. ISSN: 1069 - 4875.
Subject: 32.1901, Review: Anthropological Linguistics; General Linguistics; Sociolinguistics: Güldemann, McConvell, Rhodes (2020)
Moderator: Malgorzata E. Cavar (linguist at linguistlist.org)
Student Moderator: Jeremy Coburn, Lauren Perkins
Managing Editor: Becca Morris
Team: Helen Aristar-Dry, Everett Green, Sarah Robinson, Nils Hjortnaes, Joshua Sims, Billy Dickson
Jobs: jobs at linguistlist.org | Conferences: callconf at linguistlist.org | Pubs: pubs at linguistlist.org
Homepage: http://linguistlist.org
Please support the LL editors and operation with a donation at:
https://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/
Editor for this issue: Jeremy Coburn <jecoburn at linguistlist.org>
================================================================
Date: Tue, 01 Jun 2021 23:50:26
From: John Mansfield [jbmansfield at gmail.com]
Subject: The Language of Hunter-Gatherers
Discuss this message:
http://linguistlist.org/pubs/reviews/get-review.cfm?subid=36657137
Book announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/31/31-1884.html
EDITOR: Tom Güldemann
EDITOR: Patrick McConvell
EDITOR: Richard A. Rhodes
TITLE: The Language of Hunter-Gatherers
PUBLISHER: Cambridge University Press
YEAR: 2020
REVIEWER: John Mansfield, University of Melbourne
SUMMARY
This is a substantial edited volume focusing on a topic that has always been
in the background of anthropological linguistics, but has rarely received such
sustained attention in its own right: the linguistic and sociolinguistic
dynamics of hunter-gatherer peoples. The volume grew out of a workshop,
‘Historical linguistics and hunter-gatherer populations in global
perspective’, held at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
in 2006. Thus the gestation of the volume, which contains 23 chapters, has
taken 14 years, perhaps a fitting timescale for the subject matter of the
book.
The book is arranged into seven parts. The first part includes general
discussions of hunter-gatherer languages, while each of the remaining six
parts focuses on a particular geographic region: Africa, tropical Asia, New
Guinea and Australia, northeastern Eurasia, North America, and South America.
Part I, the Introduction, presents background discussion on the
anthropological concept of ‘hunter-gatherers’ (henceforth HGs), their genetic
distinctiveness, and the question of whether HGs are linguistically
distinctive. The first chapter is by the editors Güldemann, McConvell and
Rhodes, and provides a general discussion of HG anthropology and linguistics,
while dispelling any notion that HGs are somehow “backwards” or “relics” of
history. On the contrary, HGs are mobile, adaptive, and responsive to
intercultural dynamics with both food-producers and other HG groups.
Furthermore, cultural evolution is not a “one-way street” of progress from
foraging to food production: the authors note various instances where groups
have shifted from food production to foraging in response to ecological
changes and/or social pressures. Either food production or foraging can be
adaptive, depending on circumstances. This chapter also highlights some key
dynamics in the interaction of HGs and food producers, in particular the
tendency of food producers to push HGs into more marginal areas such as
mountains and forests, contributing to the patchwork of linguistic “spread
zones” and “residual zones” covering the earth (Nichols 2003; Nichols 2015).
Gunnrsdottir and Stoneking give a brief primer of genetic anthropology
methods, and explain why HG groups tend to be more genetically distinctive and
less internally diverse than other social groups. This genetic distinctiveness
is predominantly in the Y chromosome, because HG groups tend to be patrilocal
with a stable male core, while women more frequently marry into other groups,
including neighbouring food-producers.
Bickel and Nichols ask whether HG languages are typologically different from
those of food producers. They investigate this by running a statistical
analysis of 228 typological variables documented in two major databases,
checking for differences between the two subsistence types, while controlling
for language family and geographic area. Essentially, they do not find any
evidence for systematic differences in these typological features. However the
next chapter, by Brown, does report a difference, namely that agriculturalists
are more likely to use binomial species names, while HGs are more likely to
use atomic names. For example, Tzeltal swidden farmers have a generic name for
oak trees, hih te’, and binomial species names like bac’il hih te’ ‘Quercus
peduncularis’, sakyok hih te’ ‘Q. candicans’. By contrast, Diegueño has atomic
names such as semtaay ‘Q. chrysolepis’ and kuphall ‘Q. kelloggii’. This
difference is argued to derive from the extra attention that agriculturalists
must pay to manipulating related-but-distinct plant varieties.
Part II, on Africa, presents two case studies of specific HG groups. Savà and
Tosco discuss the Ongota people of southwest Ethiopia, a small group who rely
on a mixture of farming and fishing, but are surrounded by cattle-herding
peoples. Ongota oral history suggests that they once lived some distance away
to the north, but were forced to move several times due to conflicts over
cattle theft. The Ongota language is unclassified, though relationships to
several language families have been tentatively proposed. It is also much more
isolating, with fewer inflectional categories than other languages in the
area. The authors suggest that this linguistic profile is consistent with a
history of forced migration, in which the Ongota have been in (generally
subordinate) relationships with a series of other groups, adding successive
lexical layers to their language, and tending to reduce inflectional
morphology.
Güldemann discusses the Khoe-Kwadi languages of Southern Africa, which were
once spoken by sheep herders who moved into the area from the north-east some
2000 years ago, when there was more rainfall. Subsequent migrations by Bantu
speakers, together with desertification, forced the Khoe-Kwadi people either
to join Bantu groups or to become HGs in more marginal regions. This opened
their languages to the influence of Kx’a and Tuu languages spoken by other HG
groups in the marginal regions, leading to typological convergence between
these language families. The purported Khoisan language family is thus argued
actually to consist of three distinct lineages (Khoe-Kwadi, Kx’a, and Tuu),
which have been misidentified as a family due to contact-based convergence.
Part III, on tropical Asia, presents five case studies. Rischel discusses
Mlabri (Mon-Khmer) languages spoken by HGs in the forests of the Thailand/Laos
border. He discusses genetic, linguistic, and oral-history evidence that the
Mlabri split off from food-producing Tin people some centuries ago, shifting
to a forest-bound foraging lifeway.
Burenhult discusses the Aslian (Austroasiatic) languages spoken in the
interior forests of the Malay penninsula. He focuses on Northern Aslian
languages, spoken by Semang HG peoples. He outlines evidence suggesting that
Semang people have been continuously resident in these forests for around
fifty thousand years, and that they shifted to Austroasiatic languages due to
trading with farmers who arrived around five thousand years ago, leading to
the present-day Aslian languages. However Burenhult rejects any notion that
the Semang people can be seen as historical relics, arguing that instead they
should be seen as an “economic and genetic success story”, a people who have
been highly successful in maintaining an ecological niche in rich tropical
forests.
Blevins discusses the people of the Andaman islands, who until recently were
HGs. A salient local distinction is between coastal fishing people and inland
forest-dwellers. There are two discernable language families, Greater Andaman
and Ongan, and while Greater Andaman has many reconstructible words relating
to the ocean and fishing, Ongan instead has reconstructible forms for forest
foraging. This suggests that the two language families derived from distinct
coastal and inland ancestors.
Reid describes “Negrito” HGs of the Philippines, all of whom speak
Austronesian languages, though their habitation of the archipelago long
pre-dates the Austronesian arrival some 3500 years ago. HG languages in the
Philippines tend to reflect conservative features of the Philippine branch of
Austronesian, suggesting that they shifted to Austronesian languages fairly
soon after the Austronesians arrived.
Soriente describes HG peoples of Borneo, who speak Austronesian languages that
are all fairly similar to one another, though it remains unclear whether they
constitute a distinct phylogenetic language group. It is also unclear whether
their ancestors inhabited Borneo before food-producers arrived or whether they
are groups who split off and became foragers. The chapter also presents an
extensive historical discussion of one Borneo HG language, Penan Benalui.
Part IV, includes two chapters on New Guinea and three on Australia. Ross
discusses the spread of farming across New Guinea and nearby islands. He
describes the transition of some New Guinea groups from mobile foraging to
sedentary foraging, some twenty thousand years ago, which formed the
precondition for some groups to transition to food production in the New
Guinea highlands around nine thousand years ago. Much later, Austronesian
farmers arrived in the area, but gained a greater foothold in the Bismarck
islands (where there wasn’t already farming), compared to New Guinea itself.
Ross also briefly discusses how the highly independent villages of New Guinea,
subsisting from various mixtures of foraging and food production, maintain
high levels of linguistic diversity.
Donohue provides three case studies of pairs of neighbouring New Guinea
languages, where one group is more oriented to food production and the other
to foraging. Across the three pairs, he does not find any clear pattern of
linguistic differences between HG and food-producing groups.
Sutton discusses the historical distribution of languages in Australia, where
until recently all groups subsisted largely by foraging. He emphasises the
tendency for languages to be spoken by small numbers of people, estimating an
average of around 500-1000 individuals per language. However, he makes this
calculation for “language-owning” groups, while noting that traditional social
groups were highly multilingual, mobile bands made up of owners of several
languages. Sutton argues that small, multilingual speech communities have
typically diversified in situ over many thousands of years in Australia, while
a few rare punctuation events have periodically spread larger language
families, such as Pama-Nyungan in the mid-Holocene and Western Desert in the
last couple of thousand years. Harvey continues this theme in his chapter,
going into more detail on how Pama-Nyungan may have spread across the
continent, as well as the much smaller spread of the Mirndi language family.
McConvell rounds out this part of the book with some more detailed
reconstructions of Pama-Nyungan word histories. He focuses on meaning changes,
arguing that some Pama-Nyungan words seem to have spread from the
north-eastern part of the continent, based on semantic plausibility and
ecological relevance.
Part V, on northeastern Eurasia, consists of two chapters discussing Siberia.
Vajda discusses the Yenisean language family spoken by HGs of central Siberia,
who are surrounded by other language families (especially Turkic and Uralic)
spoken by pastoralists. He outlines evidence suggesting that Yeniseans have
been in the area for a long time and once occupied a larger territory, based
on place names and ecology-based borrowings into neighbouring languages. He
also describes structural differences between Proto-Yenisean languages
(prefixing) and neighbouring languages (suffixing), while noting some degree
of convergence in the Yenisean language Ket, which has developed some
suffixes, while avoiding lexical borrowings from neighbouring languages.
Anderson and Harrison describe the Tofa and Todzhu languages spoken by
southern Siberian peoples, who subsist from foraging assisted by their
reindeer herds. They speak Turkic languages, but these show signs of substrate
influence from an earlier language. Anderson and Harrison discuss possible
histories of language contact involving Samoyedic (Uralic) and Yenisean
languages, though the evidence available is too sparse to draw firm
conclusions.
Part VI, on North America, consists of three quite different chapters. De
Reuse revisits the “eskimo words for snow” trope with a discussion of
morphology in Central Siberian Yupik. He focuses on how “bases” and
“postbases” combine to produce word-like constructions, with the large range
of postbases accounting for the proliferation of purported words for snow.
Rhodes discusses Cree and Ojibwe sub-families of Algonquian, both spoken in
the Great Lakes region by peoples who lived primarily by foraging until
Europeans invaded. He focuses on how relocations forced by both the US and
Canadian governments led to language shifts, with some groups shifting from
one Ojibwe language to another. For example, some Potawatomi speakers were
moved to a reservation where Ottawa was the dominant language, and
subsequently shifted to Ottawa. He also highlights the spread of innovations
among these languages over the last century or so.
Hill discusses Takic and Numic branches of Uto-Aztecan, spoken by HG peoples
in southern California and the Great Basin, respectively. She notes that both
these groups were probably maize cultivators in an earlier period (as
evidenced by shared words for maize cultivation), but later shifted to
foraging when they moved into new ecological niches. She reviews previous work
suggesting that these two subfamilies exemplify distinct types of language
history: Takic peoples moved into an already-populated southern California,
and their languages diversified under contact influence from the people
already resident there; Numic peoples moved into the unpopulated Great Basin,
thus undergoing less contact and remaining more typologically homogenous. Hill
quantifies the diversity of typological traits in the two subfamilies and does
not find any difference, thus concluding that the previous proposal is
unsupported. One possible explanation is that the Great Basin was not in fact
unpopulated when the Numic people arrived.
Part VII, the final part, is on South America. Epps discusses the Nadahup
language family spoken by forest-bound foragers in the Amazonian Vaupés,
focusing on their interaction with food-producing neighbours who live along
the waterways. Nadahup people do in fact practice some light cultivation
(especially of manioc, the regional staple), and Epps examines linguistic
evidence as to the antiquity of these practices. She finds that Nadahup
languages share more words for useful wild plants and have more unrelated
words for cultivated plants, suggesting that they have taken on food
production after their language family split up, perhaps stimulated by contact
with Tucanoan and Arawakan food producers. However, there are also some shared
cultivation words in Nadahup sub-branches, suggesting that horticulture may
have been gradually integrated over a long period, rather than being a sudden
recent cultural borrowing.
Vidal and Braunstein discuss the Gran Chaco, a very sparsely documented
linguistic area in the southern part of the continent. Populations seem to
have spread into this area around six thousand years ago, and there are some
eighteen indigenous languages attested, with relations between them being
rather uncertain. The authors suggest that the standard tree model of
historical linguistics does not seem to fit these languages well, proposing
instead a “fusion/fission” model, with shared innovations suggesting a dynamic
network structure rather than discrete branching.
EVALUATION
Rather than evaluate the individual chapters on their own merits, I will
instead evaluate how well the volume fits together as a whole.
Overall, this is a fascinating volume that presents many inter-related case
studies of how language histories are shaped by HG lifeways, and especially
their interaction with neighbouring food producers. The collective force of
this volume comprehensively dismisses any notion of HGs as stagnant,
“backwards” relics of history. HGs are not frozen in time, but instead change
their economic and cultural practices in response to their ecological and
social circumstances. This includes some instances in which food producers
become HGs, if foraging becomes a more advantageous mode of subsistence.
Foraging may also be considered preferable by some groups or individuals, as
opposed to being a forced “last resort”. This is nicely illustrated in Epps’s
chapter, where she reports that Hup people actively enjoy “knocking about” in
the forest, as opposed to cultivating fields which is seen as “work”. Another
interesting illustration comes from Rischel’s chapter, where he reports that
the Mlabri people have developed a moral proscription against practicing food
production, which they say would anger their protective spirits.
Another recurrent theme is that HG and food-producer should not be
misinterpreted as mutually exclusive categories. Many of the groups described
in this volume employ some mixture of foraging and cultivation, for example,
the Ongota people’s subsistence is based on a mixture of hunting and fishing,
as well as maize cultivation and some domesticated animals (Savà and Tosco’s
chapter). Plant subsistence also involves different degrees of cultivation, as
illustrated by Papuan groups who rely heavily on sago palm carbohydrates,
which require some intervention in the life-cycle of the tree, but not to the
extent of fully fledged horticulture (Ross’s chapter). As mentioned above, the
balance of foraging/food-production also changes over time for each group of
people, and to fully understand foraging it must be seen as part of a broader
human ecology in which foragers occupy a niche and often interact with
cultivators. For example, the Pnan people (Soriente’s chapter) and Negrito
people (Reid’s chapter) trade their foraged products with agriculturalists, as
is found in many other such instances.
At the same time, this book richly illustrates the degree of prejudice to
which HG peoples are subjected. This ties together the dehumanising
anthropological discourses criticised by the editors in their introduction and
more localised prejudices found between HGs and their food-producing
neighbours. Dayak farmers sometimes claim to “own” their Pnan neighbours, who
are obliged to pay them a tithe of forest products (Soriente’s chapter).
Tukanoans farmers consider Hup HGs to be animal-like (Epps’s chapter). The
alienation of HG groups from their lands in many places, including North
America (Rhodes’s chapter) and Australia (Sutton’s chapter), has often been
justified by invading Europeans claiming that they do not have the level of
“civilisation” to be worthy of their own lands. This volume contributes to the
dismantling of these malicious untruths, and does so by linking the literature
on linguistic diversity to a more worthy depiction of HG societies.
The volume has a wide scope, comprising twenty-three chapters and 723 pages.
As such, at times it does not quite hold together as a cohesive text. The most
cohesive threads holding the volume together are those that relate HG vs
food-production to the history and structure of languages. But some chapters
have sections that depart substantially from this theme, and would probably be
more at home in some other volume. Some of these involve detailed discussion
of historical reconstruction, which is highly relevant if it tells us
something about HG history, but is otherwise not always relevant to the
volume. Overall it can be said that the majority of content in these pages
does contribute to the theme of HGs and language, but a more rigorous
editorial approach may have produced a somewhat shorter and more focused book.
Another notorious issue of edited volumes is how long they take to be
published. As mentioned above, this volume has had a long gestation, having
originated in a 2006 workshop (it can be seen as “historical linguistics” in
an unintended sense). The downside of this is of course the missed
opportunities to respond to literature that has appeared in the intervening
period. While some chapters do cite more recent work (having been written or
revised long after the workshop), others do not (quite understandably, if they
were written soon after the workshop). The last decade has seen a flourishing
of research into how the evolution of language is shaped by social structures,
and the interaction of social groups (e.g. Bowern 2010; Lupyan & Dale 2010;
Trudgill 2011; François 2014 inter alia). Within the more specific field of
each chapter, there may also be more recent work that has not been considered.
In sum, this volume illustrates the valuable contribution that can be made by
an edited volume on a well-chosen topic, while also highlighting the need to
find more efficient ways of collecting papers on a topic.
REFERENCES
Bowern, Claire. 2010. Correlates of language change in hunter-gatherer and
other “small” languages. “Language and Linguistics Compass”4(8). 665–679.
François, Alexandre. 2014. Trees, waves and linkages: Models of language
diversification. In Bowern, Claire & Evans, Bethwyn (eds.), “The Routledge
handbook of historical linguistics”, 161–189. London: Routledge.
Lupyan, G. & Dale, R. 2010. Language structure is partly determined by social
structure. PLoS ONE 5(1). 1–10.
Nichols, Johanna. 2003. Diversity and stability in language. In Joseph, Brian
D. & Janda, Richard D. (eds.), “The handbook of historical linguistics”,
283–310. Oxford: Blackwell.
Nichols, Johanna. 2015. Types of spread zones: Open and closed, horizontal and
vertical. In De Busser, Rik & La Polla, Randy J. (eds.), “Language structure
and environment: Social, cultural, and natural factors”, 261–286. Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Trudgill, Peter. 2011. “Sociolinguistic typology: social determinants of
linguistic complexity”. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
ABOUT THE REVIEWER
John Mansfield is a Lecturer in Linguistics at the University of Melbourne.
His research focuses on linguistic typology and language change, with a
particular regional focus on northern Australian Aboriginal languages.
------------------------------------------------------------------------------
*************************** LINGUIST List Support ***************************
The 2020 Fund Drive is under way! Please visit https://funddrive.linguistlist.org
to find out how to donate and check how your university, country or discipline
ranks in the fund drive challenges. Or go directly to the donation site:
https://crowdfunding.iu.edu/the-linguist-list
Let's make this a short fund drive!
Please feel free to share the link to our campaign:
https://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/
----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-32-1901
----------------------------------------------------------
More information about the LINGUIST
mailing list