16.3451, Review: Morphology/Typology: Dressler et al. (2005)

LINGUIST List linguist at LINGUISTLIST.ORG
Sat Dec 3 23:41:09 UTC 2005


LINGUIST List: Vol-16-3451. Sat Dec 03 2005. ISSN: 1068 - 4875.

Subject: 16.3451, Review: Morphology/Typology: Dressler et al. (2005)

Moderators: Anthony Aristar, Wayne State U <aristar at linguistlist.org>
            Helen Aristar-Dry, Eastern Michigan U <hdry at linguistlist.org>
 
Reviews (reviews at linguistlist.org) 
        Sheila Dooley, U of Arizona  
        Terry Langendoen, U of Arizona  

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org/

The LINGUIST List is funded by Eastern Michigan University, Wayne
State University, and donations from subscribers and publishers.

Editor for this issue: Lindsay Butler <lindsay at linguistlist.org>
================================================================  

What follows is a review or discussion note contributed to our 
Book Discussion Forum. We expect discussions to be informal and 
interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially 
invited to join in. If you are interested in leading a book 
discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available 
for review." Then contact Sheila Dooley at dooley at linguistlist.org. 

===========================Directory==============================  

1)
Date: 01-Dec-2005
From: Peter Arkadiev < alpgurev at gmail.com >
Subject: Morphology and its Demarcations 

	
-------------------------Message 1 ---------------------------------- 
Date: Sat, 03 Dec 2005 18:23:31
From: Peter Arkadiev < alpgurev at gmail.com >
Subject: Morphology and its Demarcations 
 

EDITORS: Dressler, Wolfgang U.; Kastovsky, Dieter; Pfeiffer, Oskar 
E.; Rainer, Franz
TITLE: Morphology and its Demarcations
SUBTITLE: Selected Papers from the 11th Morphology Meeting, 
Vienna, Feb. 2004
SERIES: Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 264
PUBLISHER: John Benjamins
YEAR: 2005
Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/16/16-2361.html 

Peter M. Arkadiev, Institute of Slavic Studies, Russian Academy of 
Sciences, Moscow

This volume is a collection of 18 papers presented at the 11th 
Morphology Meeting in Vienna, whose main topic was ''Demarcation 
issues in morphology: derivation vs. inflection, compounding vs. 
derivation''. Only papers explicitly dealing with the aforementioned 
topic were included in the volume. Most articles deal with data from a 
particular language, but many are typologically oriented in some 
sense or in other. The languages cited in the volume come not only 
from Europe, but also from North America, Africa, and Australia.

SYNOPSIS

''Wichita word formation. Syntactic morphology'' by David S. Rood 
argues that in Wichita, nearly extinct Caddoan language of central 
Oklahoma, there are morphological phenomena which do not fit in the 
allegedly exhaustive typology 'inflection vs. derivation vs. 
compounding', and which he proposes to call 'syntactic morphology' 
due to their direct relevance for the phrasal syntax of the language in 
question. In Wichita, a highly polysynthetic language where many 
meanings which are in more familiar languages expressed by free 
words and phrases are encoded morphologically on the verb, the verb 
has special morphemes whose function, according to Rood, is not 
related to the verb stem itself, but rather to some constituent external 
to the verb. This is illustrated by the distribution of the morpheme re:R-
 which appears in constructions meaning 'I don't know ...' and as a 
marker similar to definite article (or even, in some contexts, to 
anaphoric pronoun). Rood concludes that Wichita, which lacks 
configurational phrase structure, uses instead special kind of 
morphemes whose main function is 'to hold together the parts of an 
utterance'.

''Morphology in the wrong place. A survey of preposed enclitics'' by 
Michael Cysouw is a comprehensive survey of a rare phenomenon, 
the so called 'ditropic clitics', which are defined by the following 
characteristics: (i) their host is not characterizable neither in lexical, 
nor in syntactic terms; (ii) the constituent on the other side of the clitic, 
on the contrary, is easily definable and is functionally related to the 
clitic. In the initial sections of his paper Cysouw discusses various 
theoretical and terminological issues bearing on the topic, an 
especially notes that many previous studies denied the very existence 
of 'ditropic' clitics. Then he turns to the examples of such clitics, which 
belong to several types: (i) cross-referencing pronominal clitics (Kugu-
Nganhcara, Djinang, Kherwarian, Udi, Northern Talysh); (ii) clause-
linking clitics (Ingush, Northern Mansi); (iii) noun phrase internal clitics 
(Kwakwala, Yagua, Greek). The main conclusion Cysouw draws from 
these examples is that 'ditropic' clitics really exist, and, although rare, 
must be accounted for by any adequate theory of morphology. Then 
he considers several possible ways of explaining this phenomenon, 
both synchronic and diachronic, and notes that in many cases such 
clitics attach to some pragmatically salient or focused constituent of 
the clause.

Jasmina Milicevic in ''Clitic or affixes? On the morphological status of 
the future-tense markers in Serbian'' analyses the so called 'analytic' 
and 'synthetic' future forms of Serbian from the standpoint of the 
Meaning-Text theory (Mel'cuk 1993-2000). The formative cu that 
marks future exhibits ambiguous behaviour, but Milicevic quite 
convincingly argues that by most relevant criteria (that is, 
morphological, lexical and syntactic) it is a clitic, although from the 
point of view of morphophonemics it shows some affixal traits.

''The demarcation of morphology and syntax. A diachronic perspective 
on particle verbs'' by Corrien Blom discusses the so called 'separable 
complex verbs' (SCVs) in Dutch, which consist of a verb and of a 
preverbal element corresponding to a postposition. As is well known, 
such verbs in Dutch and its close relative German in some contexts 
split up, cf. the following examples:
<pre>
a. dat  Jan  de  boeken opzoekt
   that John the books  up-searches
   'that John looks up the books'
b. Jan  zoekt    de  boeken op.
   John searches the books  up
   'John looks up the books.'
</pre>
Blom discusses the arguments for treating SCVs as special separable 
words and shows that all the properties which purportedly indicate 
their wordhood (ability to serve as inputs to compounding and 
derivation; valency change induced by the particle; conventionalized 
meaning) are actually shared by uncontroversial syntactic phrases. 
Blom argues that SCVs are best regarded as a special case 
of 'compositional idioms' or 'idiomatically combining expressions' in 
terms of Nunberg et al. (1994), that is those phrases whose meaning 
is conventionalized but nevertheless compositionally derived from the 
meanings of their parts (e.g. 'pull strings'). Such expressions, as 
against pure idioms (e.g. 'kick the bucket') allow certain syntactic and 
lexical freedom of their parts. Finally, Blom observes that from the 
point of view of their diachronic development, Dutch SVCs are in the 
middle of the grammaticalization cline from purely syntactic phrases 
into morphologized prefixal verbs, while the degree of their 
lexicalization may vary from purely compositional and semantically 
transparent lexemes to fully idiomaticized verbs.

''When clitics become affixes, where do they come to rest? A case 
from Spanish'' by Andrés Enrique-Arias discusses a well-known 
problem of whether the order of affixes reflects the original order of 
their diachronic sources viz. free forms or clitics. The paper focuses 
on the evolution of object markers in Spanish from the 13th century to 
present, and is based on a quantified corpus study. In modern 
Spanish the position of object markers with respect to the verbal stem 
is fixed: they are prefixed to finite verb forms and suffixed to non-finite 
and imperative forms. In Old Spanish, however, the position of object 
clitics was determined by syntactic factors, mainly by the syntactic 
position of the verb, and there was quite a number of cases where 
variation in clitic placement was observed. The author then presents a 
statistical survey of a selection of Old Spanish texts and shows how 
various factors which played a role in clitic placement in Old Spanish 
gradually subsided in favour of a more rigid morphological pattern. Of 
special interest are such factors as stress placement and 'parallel 
processing effect': prefixation of object clitic was particularly favored in 
those contexts where its suffixal position would have resulted in a non-
canonical prosodic contour of the word, and there was a strong 
tendency to identically align clitics and verbs in sequences of clauses. 
The author concludes that object marker placement in Modern 
Spanish really reflects the typical order of clitics w.r.t. hosts in Old 
Spanish.

''Grammatical hybrids. Between serialization, compounding and 
derivation in !Xun (North Khoisan)'' by Bernd Heine and Christa König 
is based on the data from !Xun, a Khoisan language spoken in 
Southern Angola, northern and northeastern Namibia. This language 
has very little affixal morphology, but there is a number of serial verb 
constructions where otherwise lexical verbs lose their lexical meaning 
and become grammaticalized as markers of various grammatical 
meanings. The authors show, however, that despite their 
polyfunctionality, these items do not exhibit systematic 
morphosyntactic variability, and that whether a given occurrence of a 
verb is used as a lexical item or rather as a grammatical marker is 
determined mainly by semantic (e.g., the distinction between result 
verbs and manner verbs) and pragmatic factors. The authors 
conclude that in !Xun there is no clear-cut boundary between lexicon 
and grammar, as well as between serialization, compounding and 
derivation, since the items in question exhibit certain properties of all 
these processes. Such a situation is not unique to the Khoisan 
languages, and is attested in the languages of Southeast Asia, e.g. 
Chinese and Vietnamese. The authors suggest to model these 
phenomena in terms of 'grammaticalization chains', i.e. typologically 
attested paths of diachronic development from lexical to grammatical 
meanings (see e.g. Bybee et al. 1994, Heine, Kuteva 2002), and 
consider the situation in !Xun to be exceptional in the following 
respect: usually, semantic and morphological grammaticalization (that 
is, cliticization and affixation) go hand in hand, but here the latter 
component of the process somehow happened to not operate.

''The borderline between derivation and compounding'' by Laurie 
Bauer discusses those quite widespread phenomena (the data comes 
mainly from English, Danish, and French) which cannot be 
unambiguously classified as either derivation or compounding proper. 
First of all, Bauer notes that diachronically derivational morphology 
more often then not arises from compounding, and discusses some 
borderline cases, which can be characterized by high frequency of 
certain elements of compounds and the gradual loss of their semantic 
relationship with their lexical sources. Cases of the opposite 
development (from derivation to compounding) are, on the contrary, 
quite rare and unsystematic. Other problematic cases 
include 'synthetic compounds', whose second members (at least in 
Danish) do not usually occur as independent words, and in any case 
do not allow an uncontroversial analysis, unique morphs (such as 
English 'cran-' or 'rasp-' in 'cranberry' and 'raspberry'), 'splinters' such 
as 'burger' or '-nomics', neo-classical compounds, and finally 
prefixation in French. What characterizes all these rather discrepant 
phenomena is their being unstable diachronically, failing to maintain 
their status as independent or bound elements.

Geert Booij in ''Compounding and derivation. Evidence for 
Construction Morphology'' discusses the data more or less similar to 
that of Bauer's article (i.e., such 'borderline cases' as prefixation in 
French and Dutch, or semigrammaticalized 'affixoids' like English -
way, -wise or -like), but reaches completely different conclusions. 
Booij tries to show that, contrary to Anderson's (1992) claims, internal 
morphological structure of both compounds and derived words is 
visible to morphological and phonological/prosodic processes, and 
thus concludes that there is no such crucial difference between 
compounding and derivation that would require handling them with 
different formal tools. Then he outlines a theoretical framework which 
could capture structural similarities between compounds and affixal 
derivatives, viz. the so called 'Construction Morphology', expanding on 
the ideas of Construction Grammar, originally proposed as a non-
transformational syntactic framework (Fillmore 1988, Goldberg 1995, 
Kay 1997). Construction Morphology regards complex words of all 
types as instantiations of morphological patterns of various levels of 
generality. The most general patterns are the following, where the 
variables x, y stand for phonological strings, and the variables X, Y for 
lexical categories (N, V, A):
a. compounding:  [[x]X[y]Y]Y
b. suffixation:[[x]X y]Y
c. prefixation: [x [y]Y]Y

These patterns may be associated with certain meanings, and filled by 
particular morphological material. Thus, the general compounding 
schema is assigned the semantics 'Y with some relation to X', while 
the suffixation pattern may be instantiated by, e.g., the following 
derivational model:
[[x]Ver]N  'one who Vs'

This last pattern may be further instantiated by a particular complex 
word, e.g., 'baker' or 'worker'. This possibility of levels of 
representation intermediate between the most abstract patterns and 
concrete words is crucial of Construction Morphology, and it can 
equally apply to both derivational and compounding schemas. Booij 
discusses how various types of affixally derived and compound 
formations may be handled by the framework he proposes, dealing 
especially with the mechanisms of default inheritance between 
different levels of abstraction and of pattern unification, paying 
attention also to semantic relations between parts of complex words. 
The framework Booij proposes seems to be simple and attractive, 
allowing to capture interesting generalizations about both formal and 
semantic properties of complex words.

''Selection in compounding and derivation'' by Sergio Scalise, 
Antonietta Bisetto and Emiliano Guevara is aimed to show that the 
borderline between derivation and compounding lies not only in the 
realm of well known formal differences between these two types of 
word formation, but also in the (more or less semantically based) 
mechanism of selection of the non-head constituent of a complex word 
by its head. The authors argue that both in compounding and 
derivation there must be a process of 'head-selection', which operates 
on the basis of the Lexical-Conceptual Structure (LCS) of both head 
and potential non-head of the word. Following Jackendoff (1990) and 
Lieber (2003), Scalise et al. consider LCS of a word or a morpheme to 
consist of levels: a 'skeleton' containing such grammatically relevant 
information as syntactic category and event/argument structure, and 
a 'body' of encyclopedic features. The comparison of various types of 
derivational and compounding processes reveals the following 
systematic differences in head-selection in derivation and 
compounding:
(a) The selection operated by a derivational affix is fixed and constant, 
whereas for compounding selection is less strict: what is usually 
required is only that the non-head matches at least some information 
contained in the head's LCS;
(b) Selection in derivation is less 'syntax-like' than in compounding, 
e.g., the non-head in an affixally derived word does not satisfy any of 
the head's arguments, while in compounding this is often the case;
(c) Derived words are more predictable than compounds from both 
quantitative and semantic point of view -- it is often extra-linguistic 
information and pragmatics that play crucial role in the determining of 
the possible range of interpretations a compound may have, as well 
as its well-formedness, while for the derivational affix the determining 
factor is only whether the non-head satisfies the head's rigid 
selectional restrictions.

In ''Compounding and affixation. Any difference?'' Pavol Stekauer 
claims that from the point of view of his 'Cognitive-Onomasiological 
Model' of grammar there is no difference between compound 
formation and affixal derivation, since both involve the same 
conceptually driven processes of assigning form to conceptual 
structures. Also, from his standpoint, there is no difference between 
affixal morphemes and lexical units, because both are available to the 
process of naming and word-formation. Stekauer also proposes to 
redefine the notion 'head of the word', considering it to be semantic 
rather than formal. However, this article does not say anything about 
how the 'Cognitive-Onomasiological Model' would account for various 
purely morphological differences between (at least some types of) 
compounds and derivatives.

''On a semantically grounded difference between derivation and 
compounding'' by Bernard Fradin argues on the basis of a detailed 
analysis of French deverbal agentive derivatives in '-eur' and V-N 
compounds that there is an important semantic difference between 
affixally derived words and compounds, quite similar to what Scalise et 
al. would call difference in selectional properties. While affixal 
derivation imposes rigid constraints both on its input and its output, 
compounding requires only that the parts of a compound combine in 
such a way that best fits some semantic scenario, e.g. a 'causal 
structure' (Croft 1991). Fradin also argues that although compounding 
involves combining two lexemes, it does not result in a syntactic 
structure, since V-N compounds (contrary to the claims found e.g. in 
Di Sciullo, Williams 1987) do not form a VP structure. If they did, 
compounds such as ''marche-pied'' 'step' (lit. 'walk-foot'), where the N 
corresponds to anything but the verb's internal argument, would be 
impossible.

Dany Amiot's article ''Between compounding and derivation. Elements 
of word-formation corresponding to prepositions'' is an in-detail study 
of French complex words whose initial formative may be used as a 
preposition: ''après'' 'after', ''avant'' 'before', ''contre'' 'against', ''sans'' 
'without' etc. Amiot, using several formal and semantic criteria, shows 
that this class of formatives is not homogenous, and that some of its 
members (e.g. ''sur-'' and ''contre-'') are real prefixes exhibiting such 
properties as endocentricity, ability to combine with words of different 
syntactic categories, semantic independence from corresponding 
prepositions, while others (such as ''sans'' and ''avant'') show a weaker 
degree of grammaticalization.

''Cumulative exponence involving derivation: Some patterns for an 
uncommon phenomena'' by Davide Ricca presents some putative 
examples of cumulative exponence of derivational categories, and, 
more importantly, of derivation and inflection. The former was claimed 
(e.g. by Anderson (1992)) to be very rare, and the latter to be non-
existent. All possible examples of these rare phenomena are thus of 
great relevance for the problem of 'splitting' the morphology into two 
separate modules, viz. derivation and inflection. The data Ricca 
discusses is taken mainly from Romance languages, especially Italian, 
both literary and dialectal. Instances of cumulation in derivation usually 
involve gender or evaluative categories fused with nominalizations of 
different kinds, whereas cumulation of derivation with inflection most 
notably involves number. Ricca concludes that scarcity of cumulation 
in derivation can be related to a weaker paradigmatic structure of 
derivational processes where it is hard to establish clear-cut 
categories. On the other hand, it is possible to outline some diachronic 
sources leading to cumulation of inflection and derivation, viz. (i) 
phonological fusion across the derivation-inflection boundary; (ii) 
reanalysis of a productive derivational process, already coded 
cumulatively, into an inflectional one; (iii) grammaticalization starting 
from suppletive lexemes.

Maria-Rosa Lloret in ''Revising the phonological motivation for splitting 
morphology'' discusses some peculiar morphophonological facts from 
a Cushitic language Wellega Oromo and from Majorcan dialect of 
Catalan. In both languages a phonotactically driven process of vowel 
epenthesis happens to crucially depend on such aspects of 
morphology as nominal vs. verbal domain and inflection vs. derivation. 
Lloret argues that previous accounts of these facts in terms of cyclic 
phonological rules or underlying allomorphy are inadequate, and 
proposes an alternative treatment in terms of a correspondence 
surface-oriented Optimality Theory, using output-output 
correspondence constraints (see McCarthy 1995, Kager 1999) and 
Optimal Paradigms model (McCarthy 2005). This analysis provides 
support for the claim that surface paradigmatic relations among 
wordforms play an important role on the organization of morphology.

''Derivation versus inflection in three inflecting languages'' by Stela 
Manova deals with phenomena on the borderline between inflection 
and derivation in three Slavic languages -- Bulgarian, Russian and 
Serbo-Croatian. The processes Manova investigates are 
diminutivization, 'Movierung' (formation of nouns denoting females 
from those denoting males) and imperfectivization of verbs. Manova 
discusses various morphological, lexical and semantic properties of 
these processes and evaluates them against the common criteria for 
distinguishing between inflection and derivation, and concludes that 
according to them the phenomena in question do not show great 
discrepancy, more or less tending to the derivational pole of the 
continuum. However, according to a novel criterion proposed by 
Manova, i.e. that of inflection class assignment (in short, 'if a category 
can be identified either with a particular inflectional class or with 
complementary inflection classes, it represents (non-prototypical) 
inflection'), she assigns gender formation and imperfectivization in 
these languages to inflection.

Sergey Say in ''Antipassive sja-verbs in Russian. Between inflection 
and derivation'' explicitly states that his goal is not to assign the 
morphological phenomena he studies to either pole of this dichotomy, 
but rather to underpin those properties of various uses of the same 
affix which make it so controversial to unequivocally classify them as 
either derivational or inflectional. In the first part of the article Say 
discusses such uses of Russian sja-verbs as passive, decausative, 
reflexive, reciprocal etc. and shows that according to criteria proposed 
by Haspelmath (2002: 71) these verbs do not show uniform 
behaviour. While purely formal properties of this process seem to 
pattern with inflection, its semantic and combinatorial properties are 
more like those of derivational affixes, at least in more restricted 
reflexive, reciprocal and decausative uses (passive sja-formation are 
in most respects like inflection). The second part of the paper is 
devoted to the so called 'antipassive' uses of sja-verbs, which fall into 
two classes: 'lexical antipassives', which are unproductive and 
semantically irregular, and 'grammatical antipassives', which, despite 
important similarities with 'lexical' ones, are productive and their 
interpretation is context-dependent, not lexically restricted and 
idiosyncratic. However, Say shows that even 'grammatical' 
antipassives may become lexicalized for some speakers, and thus it is 
unreasonable to draw a strict dividing line between them. This paper 
convincingly shows that the properties that are thought as defining 
inflection and derivation can cut across not only such a polyfunctional 
process as Russian sja formation in general, but also a semantically 
homogenous phenomenon, like Russian sja antipassivization.

Rok Zaucer in ''Slavic prefixes as state morphemes. From state to 
change-of-state and perfectivity'' argues that such properties of 
prefixal verbs in Slavic languages as (i) directionality of prefixal motion 
verbs and (ii) change-of-state (perfective) meaning of prefixal 
derivatives in general can be explained if we assume that prefixes 
introduce a stative subevent into the verb's event structure, retaining 
the meanings of homophonous prepositions. Moreover, the correlation 
between a derivational prefix and verbs inflectional property of being 
perfective is only indirect and arises by regular process of event-
composition (cf. similar proposal by Pazel'skaya and Tatevosov 2005). 
Also, only those prepositions which can be used in stative copular 
constructions denoting location of an object, can have a cognate 
perfectivizing prefix, and only those prefixes which have cognate 
prepositions are actually perfectivizing. For instance, the delimitative 
prefix 'po-' is argued to derive verbs which in many respects pattern 
like imperfective rather then perfective ones. Slavic prefixation thus is 
argued to be derivational rather then inflectional.

''Delineating the boundary between inflection-class marking and 
derivational marking. The case of Sanskrit -aya'' by Gregory T. Stump 
discusses the Sanskrit suffix -aya- which is usually regarded as a 
causative morpheme. Stump tests this suffix against the following 
criteria:
(i) Distributional parallelism of inflection-class markers: If a mark 'x' of 
inflection-class membership appears in particular cells of the paradigm 
of a member of inflection class 'A' and some contrasting mark 'y' 
appears in the same cells of the paradigm of a member of some 
contrasting inflection class 'B', then 'y', like 'x', is a mark of inflection-
class membership (sufficient but not necessary property of inflection-
class markers).
(ii) Semantic contrast between derived stems and their bases: A mark 
of derivation signals a particular semantic relation between two 
lexemes. A mark of inflection-class membership does not, in itself, 
signal a particular semantic relation between two lexemes (weakly 
necessary property of derivation class markers).
(iii) Criterion of paradigmatic opposition of inflection-class markings: In 
the paradigm of a given lexeme, a mark of inflection-class membership 
may be paradigmatically opposed to another mark of inflection-class 
membership, but not to a mark of derivation (sufficient but not 
necessary property of inflection-class markers).
(iv) Criterion of uniformity of derivational markers: Marks of derivation 
are associated with whole lexemes, and therefore occur on all of the 
derived lexeme's stems. The appearance of inflection-class markers 
may be sensitive to differences among the morphosyntactic property 
sets associated with the various cells in a lexeme's paradigm 
(necessary but not sufficient property of derivational markers)

When evaluated against these criteria, -aya- turns out to be an 
inflection-class marker rather then a true derivational marker, since it 
does not appear in all wordforms of Sanskrit causative derivatives.

EVALUATION

The papers comprising the volume under review may be classified 
according to several criteria. First of all, the papers fall into three main 
groups: those which discuss the 'external' demarcation of morphology 
from syntax (Rood, Cysouw, Milicevic, Blom, Enrique-Arias, Heine and 
König), those which focus on the differences or similarities between 
compounding and derivation (Bauer, Booij, Scalise et al., Stekauer, 
Fradin, Amiot), and those dealing with the differentiation of inflection 
and derivation (Ricca, Lloret, Manova, Say, Zaucer, Stump). On the 
other hand, there are papers which argue for demarcation of such 
and such components, at least in the languages they discuss (Blom, 
Bauer, Scalise et al., Fradin, Amiot, Ricca, Manova), those, which, on 
the contrary, claim that such demarcation is unnecessary or 
impossible to draw (Heine and König, Booij), while others really focus 
not on the question 'does phenomenon A in language C belong to 
type C?' but on the very fact that the data in question do not allow 
unequivocal characterization in these terms, or on the diachronic 
issues (Cysouw, Enrique-Arrias, Say). Another important criterion is 
the nature of the argument used in the papers. Some start by showing 
that some data do not fall into any of the traditional classes by the 
commonly used criteria, but propose a novel criterion which allows 
them to assign a non-prototypical phenomenon to a certain class (e.g. 
Manova). Other present a more or less detailed analysis of more or 
less prototypical cases and argue for a more or less clear-cut 
boundary between, for instance, derivation and compounding (e.g. 
Scalise et al., Fradin). Still others propose theoretical frameworks 
which would capture similarities or differences between certain 
phenomena better then the already existing ones (Booij, Stekauer).

The main idea one may draw from the volume as a whole, abstracting 
away from the individual papers, is the following one: Demarcation in 
morphology is a controversial problem, since along prototypical 
instances of inflection, derivation and compounding showing important 
differences in morphological, syntactic, and semantic behaviour, there 
are quite a lot of borderline cases which do not allow unequivocal 
characterization since they share properties of different classes. What 
is important, then, and on what linguists should focus their attention is 
not the question of theoretical relevance and viability of the very 
notions 'inflection', 'derivation' and 'compounding', and the pursuit of 
all-or-none classification of the relevant phenomena into these three 
types, but the particular criteria and properties which underlie these 
classes, and which, as is now well known, do not always cluster in a 
straightforward way.

Finally, there are two major critical remarks I think important to make. 
The first is that it would have been useful if the authors of the papers 
have paid more attention to the work of their fellow-contributors. 
There are only a few cross-references in the volume, even when the 
authors discuss very similar phenomena, or draw similar arguments 
for their conceptions. it is particularly striking in the face of the fact 
that the volume is a collection of papers from a conference where all 
the authors were present. The second point is that the volume 
crucially lacks a large editorial introduction which would not only 
outline the structure of the volume and very succinctly summarize the 
articles, but would give a broad perspective on the problems 
discussed in the volume and of possible approaches to these 
problems, as well as some general conclusions which can be drawn 
from the discussion of individual cases. This is especially important in 
the light of the fact that the articles usually present well articulated and 
convincing arguments based on a detailed analysis of empirical data, 
arguments which, nevertheless, sometimes lead to diametrically 
opposed conclusions.

REFERENCES

Anderson, Stephen R. (1992) A-Morphous Morphology. Cambridge: 
Cambridge University Press.

Bybee, Joan L., Revere D. Perkins & William Pagliuca (1994) The 
Evolution of Grammar: Tense, Aspect and Modality in the Languages 
of the World. Chicago IL: Chicago University Press.

Croft, William (1991) Syntactic Categories and Grammatical Relations. 
The Cognitive Organization of Information. Chicago, IL: The University 
of Chicago Press.

Di Sciullo, Anna Maria & Edwin Williams (1987). On the Definition of 
Word. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fillmore, Charles A. (1988). The mechanisms of Construction 
Grammar. In Proceedings of Berkeley Linguistics Society 14, pp. 35-
55.

Goldberg, Adele E. (1995) Constructions. A Construction Grammar 
Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago, IL: The University of 
Chicago Press.

Haspelmath, Martin (2002). Understanding Morphology. London: 
Arnold.

Heine, Bernd & Tanya Kuteva (2002) World Lexicon of 
Grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Jackendoff, Ray (1990) Semantic Structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT 
Press.

Kager, René (1999) Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge 
University Press.

Kay, Paul (1997) Construction Grammar. In Paul Kay (ed.) Words and 
the Grammar of Context, pp. 123-132. Stanford, CA: CSLI.

Lieber, Rochelle (2003) Compound interpretation: Lexical semantics, 
not syntax. In G. Booij et al. (eds.), Topics in Morphology: Selected 
Papers from the 3rd Mediterranean Morphology Meeting, p. 241-253. 
IULA, Universitat Pompeu Fabra.

McCarthy, John (1995) Extensions of Faithfulness: Rotuman revisited. 
Ms., University of Massachusetts, Amherst 
(http://rucs.rutgers.edu/roa.html, ROA # 110)

McCarthy, John (2005) Optimal paradigms. In L. Downing et al. (eds.) 
Paradigms in Phonological Theory, pp. 170-210. Oxford: Oxford 
University Press (http://rucs.rutgers.edu/roa.html, ROA # 485)

Mel'cuk, Igor A. (1993-2000) Cours de morphologie générale. Vols. 1-
2. Montréal/Paris: Les presses de l'Université Montréal/C.N.R.S. 
éditions.

Nunberg, Geoffrey, Ivan A. Sag & Thomas Wasow (1994) Idioms. 
Language, 70, pp. 491-538.

Pazel'skaya, Anna & Sergey Tatevosov (2005). Uninflected VPs, 
Deverbal Nouns and Aspectual Architecture of Russian. Paper 
presented at FASL 14, Princeton University, May 2005. 

ABOUT THE REVIEWER

Peter M. Arkadiev is a PhD student and junior research fellow at the 
Department of Typology and comparative linguistics of the Institute of 
Slavic studies of the Russian Academy of Sciences, Moscow. His main 
interests are linguistic typology with focus on event and argument 
structure and its formal realization, and theoretical approaches to 
morphology. He works mainly on Lithuanian, Adyghe and Japanese.





-----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-16-3451	

	



More information about the LINGUIST mailing list