30.2945, Review: English; Syntax: Tortora (2018)

The LINGUIST List linguist at listserv.linguistlist.org
Tue Jul 30 00:54:30 UTC 2019


LINGUIST List: Vol-30-2945. Mon Jul 29 2019. ISSN: 1069 - 4875.

Subject: 30.2945, Review: English; Syntax: Tortora (2018)

Moderator: Malgorzata E. Cavar (linguist at linguistlist.org)
Student Moderator: Jeremy Coburn
Managing Editor: Becca Morris
Team: Helen Aristar-Dry, Everett Green, Sarah Robinson, Peace Han, Nils Hjortnaes, Yiwen Zhang, Julian Dietrich
Jobs: jobs at linguistlist.org | Conferences: callconf at linguistlist.org | Pubs: pubs at linguistlist.org

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org

Please support the LL editors and operation with a donation at:
           https://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/

Editor for this issue: Jeremy Coburn <jecoburn at linguistlist.org>
================================================================


Date: Mon, 29 Jul 2019 20:53:23
From: Hassan Makhad [hmakhad at hotmail.com]
Subject: Understanding Sentence Structure

 
Discuss this message:
http://linguistlist.org/pubs/reviews/get-review.cfm?subid=36502237


Book announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/29/29-3952.html

AUTHOR: Christina  Tortora
TITLE: Understanding Sentence Structure
SUBTITLE: An Introduction to English Syntax
SERIES TITLE: Linguistics in the World
PUBLISHER: Wiley
YEAR: 2018

REVIEWER: Hassan Makhad, Cadi Ayyad University

SUMMARY

This is a review of Tortora (2018), Understanding Sentence Structure: An
Introduction to English Syntax, Wiley & Sons, Inc. NJ, USA. The work is
intended as an introductory Syntax book. Its primary goal is to get readers
familiar with syntactic analysis and terminology. It consists of a preface,
acknowledgment, eleven chapters and an index. Each chapter has its own title
with detailed subsections.

Chapter One deals with the identification of the detailed aspects of
structures and how the human mind organizes speech into structured segments.
It attempts to account for the mental mechanisms which allow native speakers
to assign structures to sequences of speech parts. It shows how these
unconscious rules enable speakers to produce and understand sentences out of
word combinations. This intuitive ability is accordingly intertwined with a
biological specification of humans to learn their native languages. It thus
introduces the notion of phrase structure Rules and trees to demonstrate how
people have intuitions to associate structure with strings of words. This
proves that native speakers consider groups of words to constitute meaningful
units rather than arbitrary groupings. 

Chapter Two provides a comprehensive understanding of the concepts introduced
in Chapter One. It mainly deals with the detailed internal structure of the
Noun Phrase (NP). It offers a preliminary examination of constituent relations
between a noun and its modifiers: determiners, adjectives and prepositional
phrases. It formulates the difference between complements and adjuncts in a
very illustrative way. It maps Phrase Structure Rules into tree structures to
reflect how the human brain assembles sentential constituents. It also
addresses the notion of recursion in natural languages.

Chapter Three examines the structure of the Verb Phrase (VP). It shows that
there are many verb types. Some verbs occur with one or two objects,
prepositional phrases, or predicative adjectives. It introduces constituency
tests to demonstrate which elements form a constituent and which do not. In
addition, it addresses structural ambiguity in terms of structural hierarchy
and representations. The chapter also introduces the notion of syntactic
movement. It shows that passivization involves raising of object NPs to the
subject position.

Chapter Four studies different prepositions and types of preposition Phrases
(PP) to establish their autonomy and particularities as a distinct category.
It maintains that a PP must be headed by a P. Moreover it makes a distinction
between Phrasal Verbs (verbs followed with a preposition) and PP complements. 
In the former, P is part of the verb. In the latter, it is not. The writer
uses the particle shift test to distinguish the two constituents.

Chapter Five intends to strengthen readers’ understanding of VPs. It
establishes the concept of subcategorization.  It also introduces
deep-structure and surface-structure. The chapter introduces the
complementizer category (subordinating conjunction) and its phrase structure
representation: Complementizer Phrase (CP). It distinguishes diverse verb
types on the basis of their distinct selectional requirements. Intransitive
verbs do not select complements. Transitive verbs require one complement.
Ditransitive verbs subcategorize for two complements: a direct and an indirect
complement. It also addresses the notion of recursion in terms of copying.
When a verb selects a complement sentence, the embedded complement is a full
sentence just like the matrix sentence itself. In terms of embedding, the
embedded clause could be declarative or interrogative. In case of a
wh-construction, the wh-object raises from its deep-structure complement
position to CP. This information is part of the human linguistic knowledge of
the lexicon. 

Chapter Six turns back to NPs to deepen readers’ familiarity with complex NPs.
It shows that, similar to verbs, nouns can take sentential complements. This
similarity stems from the fact that a large number of nouns derive from verbs
on the basis of morpho-semantic operations:  she claims their claim, She
believed in his belief. Actually, this derivation does not affect the
subcategorization frame of the original word. Moreover, NPs with relative
clauses have an interesting property. The head noun of such complex NP is the
complement of the verb: I ate [the sandwichj that my mother made tj].  In the
case of the relative wh-element, it shows raising to CP: the thief [whoj  tj
stole the computer].  Examination of complex NPs has introduced traces of
movements to capture deep-structure properties of nouns.

Chapter Seven mainly deals with empty categories. These elements play
significant roles in syntax. They systematize the conceptualization of
structures in our minds. When elements move from their deep-structure
positions to their surface-structure, they leave traces behind. Co-indexation
takes care of the representation of the connection between traces and their
antecedents. In addition, the reality of empty elements is proved by
wanna-contraction. Whoj do you want tj to answer?   *Who do you wanna answer?
The ungrammaticality results as the trace of the moved element prevents wanna
contraction to operate. Another example of empty elements is the subject of
imperatives: Go there. Go there yourself. Don’t you go there. The presence of
the reflexive, yourself, indicates that there is an empty “you” in the first
sentence. This claim is proved by the existence of the same pronoun in the
negative construction. Null elements are also shown to exist in embedded
infinitives: Johnj wants proj to sing. Accordingly, the theory of silent
elements allows readers to understand the intricacies of the existence of
elements even if they are not realized phonetically. 

Chapter Eight introduces the notion of tense. This concept is relevant in the
interpretation of the grammatical meanings verbs indicate according to the
suffixes they inflect for. Tense thus specifies the location of the verb on
the timeline axis. The past denotes occurrence before the moment of speech.
The present overlaps with the utterance time. The future takes place after the
speech time. Moreover, main verbs attract tense unless they are preceded by
modals or auxiliaries. The chapter also addresses semantic and pragmatic
issues related to the interpretation of tenses and verb types. These facts
point to the human ability to use language to exactly express what they intend
to. 

Chapter Nine investigates details of verbal constructions, especially the
auxiliary verb system and its interaction with main verbs. It illustrates that
modals are distinguished from auxiliaries (be, have and do).  The latter can
act either as auxiliaries or main verbs. Modals do not have main verb
counterparts. They do not inflect for person and number and do not have
infinitival form. Their past form is ambiguous: it may express past, a
conditional or a past habitual. In addition to the main tenses
(Past-Present-Future), it discusses perfect tenses (Past perfect, present
perfect and future perfect).

Chapter Ten explores the syntax of verbs and negations. It shows that tense is
syntactically active. It is headed with tense ([±tns]) feature. It proves that
each sentence contains one single tense feature regardless of the number of
verbs it includes. It is thus associated with the leftmost verb in the
structure. In nonfinite sentence, the infinitival “to” encodes [-tns]
features. Modals are base generated in T, hence their presence denotes a
tensed sentence. Unlike modals, auxiliaries move to T across negation.
Negation is generated to the right of tense. Main verbs do not move to T in
Modern English. The tense inflections lower to them within VP. When the tense
inflection is unable to attach to a verb, do-support insertion takes place.
Its role is to grammaticalize tense and save the sentence.   

Chapter Eleven examines some impending questions and provides some attainable
answers. It introduces X-bar Theory. This Model generalizes the structure of
phrases. The sentence is accordingly a Tense Phrase (TP) projection.  The
model imposes a binary branching organization with optional multiple
bar-levels which can accommodate various adverb modifiers. It also
investigates matrix questions that require T-to-C movement. It shows that
Wh-movement involves rising to Spec. CP. Finally, it recommends readers to
follow advanced syntax studies so as to better understand the
syntactic-semantic behavior and theoretic rules of native speakers’ linguistic
knowledge.

EVALUATION

This book is a wealthy source of information about language and grammar as
well as linguistic variations among speakers of English. It begins with basic
information about sentence structure, namely that a sentence is composed of an
NP-subject and a VP-predicate. Throughout the chapters, this idea develops and
provides in-depth explorations of the internal complexities of sentential
constituents and their syntactic behaviors. 

The book is written in a simple and accessible style. This property keeps the
reader interested in exploring the complex nature of human language. The
approach makes one feel as if they are following lectures in a classroom. I
suppose this is the main strength of the work, as it keeps the reader
interested in  what comes next. In addition, it provides some interesting
characteristics in book-writing/edition. First, it gives frequent practice
exercises to make sure the studied concepts are understood. It also makes use
of “side notes” and “term boxes” to offer readers more information and
additional related knowledge with guided elaborations. 

In terms of structure, the book contains two table of content pages: a brief
one and a detailed one. The first is not probably needed. Similarly, it lacks
a page of references. Of course, the writer has provided a few references at
the end of Chapter One, Chapter eight, Chapter Ten and Chapter Eleven.
However, it is conventional to have a bibliography at the end of the book. In
addition, there are a few inaccuracies. For example, in the preface chapter,
exercises section, line 6, the word “paragraph” could be replaced by
“section.” In page 7, paragraph 2, line 2, “ This a direct result…” the verb
form “is” is missing after the demonstrative “This”. In page 25, exercise 4,
line 8, (1) must be (1b). In page 38, paragraph 2, line 5, “…as syntactic…”
should be “…a syntactic…” In page 60, exercise 5, e.g. 9, “ These students
gave her homework to the professor”. I think “ These students gave their
homework to the professor”, or “ These students gave the professor her
homework”.  Could be better. In page 66, paragraph 1, line 6, “… into related
(but different structures)..” should be “… into related (but different)
structures.” In page 236, paragraph 5, line 7, “ …we use past tense have.”
Should be “ …we use past tense had.” In page 342, paragraph 2, line 11-12,
“what will go with V” is repeated twice. 

Despite these minor typing errors, the book remains an insightful and a very
useful reference on the study of English sentence structure both for beginners
and advanced readers. The book is very informative and it is highly
recommended for instructors who teach Introduction to Syntax. I hope the
writer will produce a similar document for advanced syntax.


ABOUT THE REVIEWER

H. Makhad is a professor at Cadi Ayyad University.





------------------------------------------------------------------------------

***************************    LINGUIST List Support    ***************************
 The 2019 Fund Drive is under way! Please visit https://funddrive.linguistlist.org
  to find out how to donate and check how your university, country or discipline
     ranks in the fund drive challenges. Or go directly to the donation site:
               https://iufoundation.fundly.com/the-linguist-list-2019

                        Let's make this a short fund drive!
                Please feel free to share the link to our campaign:
                    https://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/
 


----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-30-2945	
----------------------------------------------------------






More information about the LINGUIST mailing list