31.1340, Review: Cognitive Science; Language Acquisition; Psycholinguistics: Sharwood Smith, Truscott (2019)

The LINGUIST List linguist at listserv.linguistlist.org
Tue Apr 14 02:17:47 UTC 2020


LINGUIST List: Vol-31-1340. Mon Apr 13 2020. ISSN: 1069 - 4875.

Subject: 31.1340, Review: Cognitive Science; Language Acquisition; Psycholinguistics: Sharwood Smith, Truscott (2019)

Moderator: Malgorzata E. Cavar (linguist at linguistlist.org)
Student Moderator: Jeremy Coburn
Managing Editor: Becca Morris
Team: Helen Aristar-Dry, Everett Green, Sarah Robinson, Lauren Perkins, Nils Hjortnaes, Yiwen Zhang, Joshua Sims
Jobs: jobs at linguistlist.org | Conferences: callconf at linguistlist.org | Pubs: pubs at linguistlist.org

Homepage: http://linguistlist.org

Please support the LL editors and operation with a donation at:
           https://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/

Editor for this issue: Jeremy Coburn <jecoburn at linguistlist.org>
================================================================


Date: Mon, 13 Apr 2020 22:16:43
From: Marta De Pedis [marta.depedis at gmail.com]
Subject: The Multilingual Mind

 
Discuss this message:
http://linguistlist.org/pubs/reviews/get-review.cfm?subid=36582218


Book announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/30/30-3451.html

AUTHOR: Michael  Sharwood Smith
AUTHOR: John  Truscott
TITLE: The Multilingual Mind
SUBTITLE: A Modular Processing Perspective
PUBLISHER: Cambridge University Press
YEAR: 2019

REVIEWER: Marta De Pedis

SUMMARY

This book is a detailed description of the MOGUL (Modular On-line Growth and
Use of Language) framework. Such framework consists of a modular architecture
of the mind such that most (or all) of its mechanisms can be explained in a
parsimonious way. However, the authors mostly focus on describing how language
processing works, and in particular how two or more languages interact and
share space in the users’ minds.

The book opens with a useful introduction on MOGUL (Chapter 1: pp. 1-26) and
is then divided into two parts. Part I (“The framework”: pp. 27-180) is
devoted to describing the architecture of MOGUL and language acquisition.
Chapter 2 (pp. 29-63) describes the language module as proposed by the
authors, and Chapter 3 (pp. 64-92) describes language processing in MOGUL. In
Chapter 4 (pp. 93-136), the authors describe the mechanisms underlying
language growth and the creation of new representations, focusing on the
syntactic module. Chapter 5 (137-177) aims to describe the overall
architecture of the mind, its processes and growth.

Part II (“Applying the framework”: pp. 181-340) focuses on the bilingual mind
and the role of consciousness and attention on language acquisition and
learning. Chapter 6 (pp. 181-211) is devoted to describing how different
languages share mind space and interact within MOGUL. Chapter 7 (pp. 212-251)
turns again to language development, mirroring Chapter 4, but this time in a
multilingual mind. Chapter 8 (pp. 252-279) deals with the role of
consciousness and attention as crucial components of the cognitive
architecture, and Chapter 9 (pp. 280-307) looks at how these influence first
and second language acquisition and learning. In Chapter 10 (pp. 308-340), a
range of phenomena on second language learning is addressed and fitted in
MOGUL. Chapter 11 (pp. 341-360) outlines general conclusions and is followed
by the references and the alphabetical index.

Chapter 1: Introduction (pp. 1-26). The aim of this chapter is to describe
what MOGUL is (whereas in Chapter 2 a more detailed description is given), as
well as to clarify the theoretical framework in which it emerged. At first,
some terminological clarifications are given. Then the authors proceed to
step-by-step describe the basics and the core components of MOGUL, a
tripartite system based on Jackendoff (2002)’s modular architecture. Each part
of this modular system is in charge of creating representations specific to
their own module (e.g., a syntactic representation for the syntactic system,
as opposed to a phonological representation created by the phonological
system). Each module is then linked to the next one by means of a specialised
interface, in a chain-like fashion. In the following paragraphs, the authors
pose MOGUL in the theoretical context it has risen, and elaborate their
approach to language processing. While doing so, they also justify some of the
theoretical choices taken. The chapter closes with a useful bullet-point
summarisation.

Chapter 2: The language module: architecture and representations (pp. 29-63).
This chapter aims at describing in more detail what had been introduced in the
previous chapter. It opens with an explanation of what modularity is, and what
the characteristics of MOGUL are. It then follows with a description of the
actual language modules and processors of MOGUL and their functioning. The
authors then proceed to describe the nature of linguistic representations
(i.e., combinations of primitive phonological, syntactic and conceptual items
into a more complex item) and their interconnection between the three
linguistic modules. Finally, they tackle the topic of working memory and its
implementation in MOGUL.

Chapter 3: Processing in the language module (pp. 64-92). This chapter is
devoted to describing some main themes, findings and models in the language
processing literature, and fitting them into MOGUL’s processing mechanism.
Therefore, the authors first describe some major linguistic processing issues
(e.g., competition, activation, serial versus parallel processing etc.), the
evidence collected on their regard, and finally the solutions that had been
proposed. Then, they proceed to describe processing in MOGUL in light of each
of these findings, explicitly describing how it will approach each of the
topics proposed above, and suggesting “a form of incremental processing
involving competition between candidate items (tentatively interpreted in
terms of a race), in which success is based directly on current activation
level and indirectly on both resting level and suitability for the current
processing activity. […] Also, although modular, it leaves considerable room
for constrained influences across modules […].” (pp. 72). Examples for the
processing of two sentences are provided.

Chapter 4: Growth of the language module: acquisition by processing (pp.
93-136). In this chapter, the authors describe how the language modules
acquire new representations. The approach used is called APT “Acquisition by
Processing theory”, whose essence lies in the following maxim: “Acquisition is
the lingering effects of processing”. That is, within MOGUL, learning is
treated as a normal means of processing a new input: the framework does not
provide any special learning mechanism. When a new word is encountered, the
processors build a new item and its adequate connections. The new item is then
stored in the long-term memory at a very low resting level. When it is
activated again, the resting level is raised and said item will be more and
more available for future selections. This results in a stable set of highly
active items. The chapter describes such an approach, justifies it, and gives
a few examples, focusing on the syntactic level.

Chapter 5: Beyond the language module (pp. 137-177). In this chapter, the
authors suggest that the whole cognitive system works in a similar way to the
language system. Therefore, they describe the overall architecture of the mind
as a set of specialised processing units. These units are in principle similar
to the ones described earlier, although some differences are accounted for (so
that each unit can adapt to its specific tasks). The authors then describe
some of the hypothesised processing units (e.g., perceptual, conceptual,
affective units). Finally, they look at different varieties of knowledge
(i.e., non-linguistic knowledge, metalinguistic knowledge, word meaning,
orthography) and their growth within the framework.

Chapter 6: The bilingual mind introduced (pp. 181-211). Section II (“Applying
the framework”) opens with this chapter. The authors describe how the mind
handles multiple languages within the MOGUL framework. They suggest the
Conceptual Triggering Hypothesis, in which the processors for phonology and
syntax do not discern whether they are processing one language or another.
That is, the syntactic and phonological representations have no “language tag”
attached to them. Instead, the identity of the language is determined by the
conceptual module, which receives information directly from the sound (or
visual) appearance of the language before it is processed. That is: we know
some word or sound is in French without having to process it, just because it
looks like French and it is different from any other language present in our
mind. This is why, for example, we are able to recognise that a text is from a
certain language, even without knowing that language. Therefore, while the
language system blindly processes the input, the conceptual system activates a
single representation of the language under account, causing a spread
activation of all the concepts associated with it. Consequently, all the, say,
French items will be much more active than the English ones. Conveniently,
under this account, no language tags are needed on each processing level, and
the concept of inhibition becomes unnecessary (the supposedly inhibited
language is just a collection of representations which fail to reach enough
activation to compete with the active language). The authors then proceed to
describe code-switching and conscious bilingual processing (i.e., translation
and interpreting).

Chapter 7: The growth of a second language (pp. 212-251). In this chapter, the
authors present some approaches in second language research and describe how
they fit into the MOGUL framework. Following, they describe second language
growth within the phonological, syntactical and conceptual systems, focusing
on how items in different languages compete in the same mind. The approach is,
again, the APT “Acquisition by Processing Theory” (as seen in Chapter 4).
Finally, they discuss metalinguistic knowledge and language attrition.

Chapter 8: Consciousness and attention (pp. 252-279). In this chapter, the
authors define conscious representations (of any kind) as those
representations on perceptual or affective stores that have a sufficiently
high activation level. They proceed to describe this account in detail.
Following, they discuss the notion of attention and provide two different
accounts in order to describe its role in cognitive processes. While the
limited resources account can be fit into MOGUL, still they adopt the view
that attention is not an independent theoretical entity: instead, it would be
a mere result of the overall cognitive mechanisms.

Chapter 9: The role of consciousness in language growth (pp. 280-307). The
authors here consider the role of consciousness in the growth of the language
modules for either a first or second language. First, they turn to which kind
of linguistic representations could be conscious and conclude that syntactic
and phonological items cannot reach awareness. Then, they describe how
conscious learning plays a role in first and second language development, and
define the notion of implicit learning within MOGUL. Finally, they turn to
some considerations about second language teaching and the implications of
this framework on it.

Chapter 10: Issues in SLA revisited (pp. 308-340). In this chapter, the
authors select five recurrent topics in second language acquisition and fit
them into MOGUL. Therefore, they turn to the debate on whether language
learning is best seen as a continuous or a staged process, and they show how
MOGUL can accommodate and reconcile both notions. Then they address the topics
of crosslinguistic influence, optionality, native-like proficiency in a second
language (“ultimate attainment”), and anxiety in language learning.

Chapter 11: Conclusion (pp. 341-360). The authors here summarise the main
concepts described in the book. They firstly reiterate the core architecture
of MOGUL. Then, they address the topic of the specificity of human language,
arguing that it is plausible that animals at least partially share the basic
cognitive architecture (as described in Chapter 5) with humans. Following,
they turn to redefining some recurring concepts in language research and
framing them into MOGUL (e.g., What is a representation in MOGUL? What is
executive function?), partially reiterating and specifying what had been
already explained in the previous chapters. Finally, a few comments about the
still to be uncovered neural correlates of MOGUL are made, in order to trigger
further research on the topic.

EVALUATION

The book offers an extensive description of the MOGUL (Modular On-line Growth
and Use of Language) framework. The proposal is interesting, parsimonious, and
elegant. The book is easy to follow, clearly written, and well organised
(notably, each chapter opens with a chapter outline and concludes with a
useful chapter summary), and the exposition is coherent. The authors manage to
build up the information about their proposal in a step-by-step fashion,
sometimes reiterating previously seen concepts for the sake of clarity.

Furthermore, the book is well fit into its theoretical framework: it
explicitly states and discusses its theoretical antecedents, and broadly
covers the relationship of MOGUL with other theories and hypotheses about
language processing. However, it has to be noted that such concurrent
hypotheses are not widely described. For this reason, I do not think the book
is suitable for a naïve audience, given that it requires quite some knowledge
of previous perspectives and proposals. I have my doubts about whether it
could be suitable for master (graduate) students that are not well-grounded
into psycholinguistics and cognitive linguistics.

Another interesting feature is that the authors address a broad variety of
extensively discussed topics in language research (e.g., the influence of
working memory in language processing, first versus second language learning
etc.).

Furthermore, and as the authors state in the conclusive chapter of the book,
their proposal could potentially trigger future research, mainly in terms of
looking for the neural correlates of MOGUL, and of further specifying the
model itself within less extensively discussed areas (such as, for example,
cognitive processes unrelated to language).

To sum up, if anyone is interested in understanding MOGUL’s framework, how it
works, and where it is posited in the debate about language and cognition, I
strongly recommend this book.

However, I have to raise a few comments.

Firstly, in a great number of passages, the authors describe their aim as to
fit MOGUL with as many as possible other approaches. By doing so, they create
a highly flexible model which can accommodate almost any other approach,
finally not taking any position nor clarifying a few issues. As an example,
take the case of syntactic categories (p. 44). When describing what a
syntactic primitive category is, the authors report that there has been some
disagreement in the literature about how many of these can exist in a human
mind. After presenting two different positions on the issue, they finally
claim that: “We will not take a position here on this question. Again, the
MOGUL framework is compatible with a variety of specific theories and, as
elsewhere, this fact should be kept in mind when examining our examples.” That
is to say, in some cases the authors seem more eager to produce a
comprehensive account of all theoretical approaches (resulting in a somewhat
blurry model), rather than seeking to describe the actual architecture of the
mind as based on empirical data.

In fact, it is my opinion that the authors’ approach is not at all empirical.
That is, their book does not point to significant experimental works that test
their framework’s validity. This might be a common reaction, given that on
page 342 they claim: “…the framework can already be said to have considerable
empirical foundation and certainly enough to generate further interesting and
researchable questions. We find it important to stress this since a common and
not unexpected response on being introduced to MOGUL is ‘what’s your
evidence?’ and ‘what are your predictions?’” Following, they claim that MOGUL
is an integration of previous and diverse approaches which are already backed
up by empirical evidence. However, not much of such data is offered to the
reader. Instead, many theoretical possibilities are presented as equally
possible, in light of conflicting evidence (as it is common in language
research). Furthermore, I believe that experimental works that are not aimed
at specifically testing a hypothesis are not unequivocally in favour of such a
proposal. That is, if a decade-old peer-reviewed paper’s results fit into
MOGUL’s framework, it does not mean that such a framework has been validated.
It just means that MOGUL is a proper candidate for describing how the mind
works, and that it could be as good as a model as a bunch of others (the
authors seem to suggest this as well, still on page 342: “We do not claim it
[MOGUL] is the only possible account, only that it is a plausible one…”). I
believe that the lack of specific empirical research is a void to be filled in
future updates to this work.

Moreover, and in relation to this last topic, how could this framework just be
validated or falsified? Can we actually test the authors’ proposal, and how?
What predictions can be made in order to shed light into the validity of
MOGUL? What repercussions does this book have in empirical language research?
These are some of the questions that crowd my mind, to which the authors could
not give an answer. However, I believe these are indeed questions that are to
be necessarily asked in order to substantiate such an elegant proposal.

REFERENCES

Jackendoff, R. 2002. Foundations of language. Oxford University Press.


ABOUT THE REVIEWER

Marta De Pedis is a PhD student in psycholinguistics at the University of
Basque Country. Her thesis deals with relative clause processing and relative
clause attachment preferences. Her main interests are syntactic processing and
early bilingualism.





------------------------------------------------------------------------------

***************************    LINGUIST List Support    ***************************
 The 2019 Fund Drive is under way! Please visit https://funddrive.linguistlist.org
  to find out how to donate and check how your university, country or discipline
     ranks in the fund drive challenges. Or go directly to the donation site:
               https://iufoundation.fundly.com/the-linguist-list-2019

                        Let's make this a short fund drive!
                Please feel free to share the link to our campaign:
                    https://funddrive.linguistlist.org/donate/
 


----------------------------------------------------------
LINGUIST List: Vol-31-1340	
----------------------------------------------------------






More information about the LINGUIST mailing list