Deixis, Buhler and the Problem of Ambiguity
Daniel Everett
dlevere at ilstu.edu
Mon May 18 16:53:59 UTC 2009
And of course Mike Tomasello (whom Hrdy acknowledges) has been talking
about this stuff for years, looking at cross-species data among
different primates.
Dan
On 18 May 2009, at 12:39, Tom Givon wrote:
>
> I think that before we accept as gospel the idea that "from the
> infant's point of view, deixis is as confusing as a hall of
> mirrors", we ought perhaps look a bit more carefully at how infants
> actually acquire communicative expression of reference, starting
> from deixis and going on to other kinds of reference. The CHILDES
> transcript of communication in the first year of life shows that the
> prerequisite to reference is the care-giver's intense exercise in
> establishing JOINT ATTENTION. The reason why this will become first
> deictic reference is obvious--in early childhood, all communication
> is about here-and-now, you-and-I, this-and-that accessible to both
> of us in the shared speech situation. There is nothing confusing to
> the infant in these learning sessions. On the contrary, the process
> capitalizes on the shared perceptual field and the child's innate
> propensity to attend to salient objects--colorful, compactly-shaped,
> fast-moving, or pointed to by the care-giver. But the child is also
> acquiring another important prerequisite to reference--and
> communication in general--during the first year of life: Considering
> OTHER MINDS as having a perspective distinct from one's own (inter-
> subjectivity; theory of mind). So the acquisition of referential
> communication is deeply embedded in these early capacities. Joint-
> attention sessions are indeed early theory-of-mind instructional
> sessions.
>
> Attracting the child attention to a referent within the shared
> situation in early childhood is done by various pointing means--
> touching, approaching, holding-bringing-and-showing, changing the
> child's position, pointing, and eventually verbal deictic
> expressions. Verbs of perception such as "see", "look", "ear" or
> "touch" are prominently used in the care-giver's verbal "obligato"
> that accompanies these joint-attention (or joint-reference)
> sessions. Early nominal vocabulary is also prominently introduced at
> these sessions. And early uses of determiners ('this', 'the',
> 'your', 'my') that are not motivated by discourse but still by the
> deictic situation.
>
> With the gradual change during the second year to communication
> about non-present objects and future and past events, the move from
> deictic to other types of reference is phased in, together with more
> sophisticated grammatical devices that point at remembered or
> imagined referents. Thus, while the domain of reference expands, the
> basic principle established in early infancy--JOINT-ATTENTION--
> remains as the leitmotif of all referential gestures, verbal &
> otherwise: Make sure that you & I are attending to the same thing.
> This is, of course, deeply embedded in the human capacity to
> consider other minds ("inter-subjectivity, Theory of Mind, empathy).
> There is a beautiful recent book by Sarah Hrdy on the evolution of
> this capacity ("Mother & Others") that I think is perhaps worth
> reviewing here, maybe later. (And Ch. 8 "How children acquire
> complex reference" of my recent "The Genesis of Syntactic
> Complexity" deals in some detail with the child reference data
> during years 2-3-4).
>
> Cheers, TG
>
> ==============
>
>
> In more sophisticated referential learning during the 2nd and 3rd
> year,
>
> Salinas17 at aol.com wrote:
>> In a message dated 5/18/09 3:17:34 AM, twood at uwc.ac.za writes:
>> --I agree with the broad notion of deixis; I have never thought of
>> it as a early stages ("see the kitty?").
>> small class of linguistic expressions. But I don't agree that it
>> has much to do with ambiguity. It seems to me that deixis is more
>> like the pole of concrete as opposed to abstract in language, or
>> specific as opposed to universal. So a linguistic expression will
>> tend to have a deictic content as well as an ideational content--
>>
>> Tahir - Thanks for the comment. Let me suggest that ambiguity
>> arises in two ways with deixis. One is the simple problem created
>> by external context. Levinson describes these on all levels, but
>> the most apparent are the most basic -- "from the infant’s point of
>> view, deixis is as confusing as a hall of mirrors: my “I” is your
>> “you”, my “this” is your “that”, my “here”
>> , your “there”, and so forth."
>>
>> Ambiguity is also involved with deixis when we use it to be
>> definite, i.e., to minimize ambiguity -- I don't want any car but
>> this car. The irony here is that what decreases ambiguity also
>> increases ambiguity, since we are not in Kansas anymore when we
>> accept deictic reference into our study of expression.
>>
>> The problem I cited with deixis applying to abstracts is that we
>> really have no way of stopping the ball at just concretes. For
>> example:
>> John knew that.
>> That was exactly what I was thinking.
>> Do you believe this?
>> Here is where we part thinking.
>> That is diectic and this is not.
>> Here, on the other hand, a squared times b squared equals d.
>>
>> So-called secondary deixis apparently can apply to extreme
>> abstracts -- which is why perhaps Buhler limited deixis to the
>> point before the "pointing" became representation or symbolic.
>> Perhaps because the process changes after that, if we are pointing
>> to an abstract.
>>
>> regards and thanks,
>> steve long
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>> **************
>> A Good Credit Score is 700 or Above. See Yours in Just 2 Easy
>> Steps! (http://pr.atwola.com/promoclk/100126575x1221322941x1201367178/aol?redir=http://www.freecreditreport.com/pm/default.aspx
>> ?sc=668072&hmpgID=115&
>> bcd=Mayfooter51809NO115)
>>
>>
>
More information about the Funknet
mailing list