numerals and number markers

Edith A Moravcsik edith at CSD.UWM.EDU
Wed Feb 11 21:14:40 UTC 1998


      Dear ALT Colleagues,

      Recently, the four of us - Grev Corbett, Alan Dench, David Gil, and
      Edith Moravcsik - became engaged in an e-mail discussion
      about criteria that may serve to differentiate between
      numerals and quantifiers, on the one hand (e.g. "two" or "many")
      and grammatical number markers, on the other (i.e., dual, trial,
      or plural markers). The discussion was triggered by Grev's paper
      on grammatical number given at the ALT-II meeting in Eugene, Oregon,
      this past September and started by David, who called attention
      to some constructions from English and Hebrew which seemed to
      be borderline cases between involving a numeral/quantifier
      and a grammatical number marker. For example:

        English:     we two
                     we three

        Hebrew:      SloS-t-enu          "three-CONSTR-1:PL"   'we three'


      Further examples from other languages:

        Hungarian:   (mi-)kett-en        "we-two-AFF"          'we two'
                     (mi-)ketto"-nk-et   "we-two-lPL-ACC"      'us two'

        Yinhawangka: ngunha-kutharra-ma  "that-two-EMPH"       'those two'

        Breton:      hon-daou            "us/our-two"          'we two'
                     ho-tri              "us/our-three"        'we three'

         (Note: re Hebrew:    hyphen placement is a rough approximation
                re Hungarian: the diacritic " should go on the preceding
                              vowel)

      In each case, the question is the same: are these words number-marked
      forms of pronouns or are they numerals showing person and number?

      The conclusion that we tentatively settled on was the conventional
      one: free versus bound status. That is:

      a/ BOUNDNESS CRITERION
         If the quantifying formative is a free form, it is a numeral or
         quantifier; if the formative is an affix, it is a
         grammatical number marker.

      By this criterion, all the forms above are inflected numerals
      rather than number-marked pronouns since the quantifying formative
      they include is in each case a free form. Note that the
      boundness criterion does not always provide for a clear cut: it
      leaves open the analysis of forms like Tagalog _mga_ and that of
      "plural words" (Dryer 1989).

      In the course of the discussion, we tried other possible criteria
      as well. These were the following:

      b/ CARDINALITY
         Does the quantifying formative express cardinality or not?
         If it does, it is a numeral; if it does not, it is
         a grammatical  number marker.
         This is clearly in conflict with the boundness criterion:
         formatives expressing cardinality may be inflections
         (such as dual or trial markers) and formatives without
         cardinality may be free forms (such as English _few_ or
         _many_).

      c/ LOWER NUMBERS?
         Assuming that cardinality (as in b/) is not the right criterion
         so that both numerals/quantifiers and grammatical number
         markers may or may not express cardinality, a partially
         applicable criterion may be whether a quantifying formative
         that does have cardinality is or is not restricted to lower
         numbers. If it is, the formative is a grammatical number
         marker; if it is unrestricted in terms of how high the number
         can be, it is a numeral.

         This criterion does not support the boundness criterion for the
         English, Hebrew and Hungarian constructions cited above. These
         constructions involve free-form numerals; yet, they must be
         lower numerals. Also, there are languages where free-form
         numerals never go above 5 or 10.

      d/ HEAD OR DEPENDENT?
         Is the quantifying formative a head or a dependent? If it is a
         head, it is a numeral or quantifier; if it is a dependent, it
         is a grammatical number marker.

         This is again in conflict with the boundness criterion. In the
         Hungarian examples above, the numeral is a free form and the
         pronominal element is an affix and thus the boundness
         criterion would say these words are inflected numerals, But,
         interestingly, if the word serves as a controller of agreement
         (with the verb agreeing with it as subject or as object, or the
         reflexive pronoun agreeing with it as subject), the verb and
         the reflexive show the person and number of the pronoun, rather
         than being in the third person plural as one would expect if
         the numeral were the controller and thus the head.

      e/ STACKING
         If the construction involves more than one quantifying
         formative, is their interpretation flat or hierarchical? If it
         is flat, such as in English _two eyes_, one of the quantifying
         formatives is a grammatical number marker and the other, a
         numeral or quantifier. If the interpetation is hierarchical
         (i.e., the two multiply each other, such as in Breton _daou-
         lagad-ou`_ "DUAL-eye-PLURAL"  meaning 'pairs of eyes'; for
         other similar cases, cf. Japanese), both are
         numerals/quantifiers.

         This criterion supports the boundness criterion in some
         instences. For example, English _two eyes_ has two quantifying
         elements (_two_ and _-s_) and a flat interpretation; and it is
         true that one of the quantifying elements is free and the
         other is bound. Thus _two_ classified as a numeral and _-s_
         as a grammatical number marker is consistent with both
         criteria.

         In other instances, however, the criteria of stacking and
         boundness are in conflict. For example, in the above-cited
         expression of Breton (_daou-lagad-ou`_ "DUAL-eye-PLURAL" 'pairs
         of eyes'), stacking would rule both the dual and the plural
         element numerals since the interpretation is hierarchical; but
         boundness would rule both as grammatical number markers since
         both are affixes.

         Another problematic case is Georgian. The following Georgian
         phrase is ambiguous:

            xut-i   or-or-i       vaSl-i
            ten-ABS two-DISTR-ABS apple-ABS"

         The above phrase may be interpreted either hierarchically or
         flatly. The flat interpretation is 'ten apples in groups of
         two' - a total of ten apples; the hierarchical (or stacked)
         interpretation is 'ten sets of pairs of apples' - a total of at
         least twenty apples. As far as the status of the two
         quantifying elements _xut-i_ 'ten-ABS' and _or-or-i_ "two-
         DISTR-ABS" is concerned, the stacking criterion makes different
         predictions depending on the interpretation. Under the
         hierarchical interpretation, both would have to be numerals and
         this is so far in line with the boudness criterion since both
         are free forms. Under the flat interpretation, however, the
         stacking criterion would class either _or-or-i_ or _xut-i_
         'ten' as a grammatical number marker even though they are both
         words.

      f/ ANIMACY HIERARCHY
         Do the cooccurrence constraints between the quantifying
         formative and the noun or pronoun it goes with follow
         the version of the animacy hierarchy as proposed by T. Cedric
         Smith-Stark (1974) for predicting the distribution of
         grammatical plural markers? If it does, it is a grammatical
         number marker; if it does not, it is a numeral or quantifier.

         This criterion conflicts, at least in some cases, with the
         boundness criterion. For example, the English, Hebrew and
         Hungarian forms cited above include a free numeral but the
         construction is nonetheless restricted to pronouns - a
         constraint that the Smith-Stark hierarchy would predict to hold
         for grammatical number markers rather than for numerals.

       At this point, our discussion petered out. There are
       nonetheless some further questions that we see as wide open:

         A. Are the criteria listed above reasonable to begin with?
         B. The criteria clearly do not always coincide. To exactly what
            extent do they converge and diverge? Are there any
            implicational relations that could be set up as holding
            among them?
         C. Are there any other criteria that one could rely on to
            tell apart grammatical number markers and
            numerals/quantifiers? (For example, linear order relative
            to the noun or pronoun?)
         D. Is it necessary to draw a distinction between quantifiers/
            numerals and number markers to begin with?

       We hope that you will want to offer some ideas on these issues
       or ask us any questions about the above.

                    Grev Corbett (g.corbett at surrey.ac.uk)
                    Alan Dench (adench at cyllene.uwa.edu.au (Alan has
                      not had a chance to approve of this summary)
                    David Gil (dgil at strauss.udel.edu)
                    Edith Moravcsik (edith at uwm.edu) (in charge of
                      the write-up)

       References:

         Dryer, Matthew. l989. "Plural words." _Linguistics_ 27. 865-
            895.

         Smith-Stark, T. Cedric. l974. "The plurality split." Papers
            from the Tenth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society.
            657-671. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.










					      	



More information about the Lingtyp mailing list