LEXICON-IN-CONTACT
Maria Koptjevskaja Tamm
tamm at LING.SU.SE
Tue Dec 18 10:07:34 UTC 2012
Call for papers for the thematic workshop at the SLE (Societas
Linguistica Europea) 2013 conference in Split, September 18-21
(http://www.sle2013.eu/)
LEXICON IN CONTACT: CONTACT-INDUCED STRUCTURAL ISOMORPHISM IN THE LEXICON
Workshop organizers
Maria Koptjevskaja Tamm (tamm at ling.su.se <mailto:tamm at ling.su.se>),
Henrik Liljegren (henrik at ling.su.se <mailto:henrik at ling.su.se>), Maarten
Mous (M.Mous at hum.leidenuniv.nl <mailto:M.Mous at hum.leidenuniv.nl>),
Matthias Urban (urbanm at staff.uni-marburg.de
<mailto:urbanm at staff.uni-marburg.de>)
Topic description
The workshop will focus on different structural outcomes of prolonged
language contact in the realm of the lexicon. Lexical phenomena have of
course a long standing record in research on language contact; however,
the recent developments in areal linguistics and areal typology have,
with a few exceptions, mainly concerned grammatical phenomena. This is
not at all surprising given the central place of research on grammar in
the modern linguistics of all denominations, including typology.
The two traditionally distinguished groups of contact phenomena in the
lexicon are loanwords and calques, or semantic loans -- the distinction
paralleled by contact phenomena at other levels ('replication of matter'
vs. 'pattern replication' in Matras and Sakel 2007). Loanwords have been
studied from a more systematic cross-linguistic perspective, where the
core issue has been different borrowability of different words, seen as
belonging to different parts of speech and/or coming from different
semantic domains (cf. Haspelmath and Tadmor eds. 2009). These are good
examples of questions where research on language contact shares common
interests with lexical typology, by which we mean the cross-linguistic
and typological branch of lexicology. The interesting research angles
here, as elsewhere in research on contact phenomena and in
(areal-)typological research (cf. Koptjevskaja-Tamm 2011), are possible
outcomes of language contact in the realm of the lexicon, on the one
hand, and a possibility of using lexical phenomena for reconstructing
contact, on the other.
But a lexical-typological contribution to contact linguistics has an
even greater potential when it comes to pattern replication rather than
to replication of matter. Hayward (1991, 2000, cf. also Treis 2010)
points out many shared lexicalization patterns in the three Ethiopian
languages Amharic (Semitic), Oromo (Cushitic) and Gamo (Omotic), which
add to the cumulative evidence in favour of the Ethiopian linguistic
area and fall into four categories: (i) shared semantic specializations,
e.g. 'die without ritual slaughter (of cattle)'; (ii) shared polysemy,
e.g. 'draw water' -- 'copy'; (iii) shared derivational pathways, e.g.
'need' = causative of 'want': (iv) shared ideophones and idioms, e.g.,
'I caught a cold' being expressed via 'a cold caught me'. François
(2011) describes the pervasive "structural isomorphism" in the lexicon
of the closely related northern Vanuatu languages whereby many words,
formally not related to each other, "display the same properties across
languages: the same semantic range (polysemy, polyfunctionality), the
same combinatorics, and parallel usage in phraseology", and the
challenge this situation offers for historical linguistics. Enfield
(2011) suggests that the lexical semantic domain of taste in the
genetically unrelated SEA languages Lao (Tai) and Kri (Austronesian)
show striking similarities in their conceptual organization, which might
perhaps be accounted for by convergence. Matisoff (2004), Vanhove (ed.
2008), Zalizniak et al. (2012) and Urban (2012) give numerous examples
of cross-linguistically recurrent patterns of polysemy (e.g., 'eat' -->
'suffer') and lexical motivation ('wind-faeces' for 'cloud' in New
Guinea), some of which are clearly areally restricted and witness of
language contact, whereas others rather reflect universal tendencies.
We invite contributions dealing with different kinds of contact induced
structural isomorphism in the lexicon, as manifested in the general
organization of a lexical field, polysemy patterns and lexical
motivation, collocational patterns etc. We are particularly interested
in contributions that have a scope of an area or a larger number of
languages and make an attempt at generalizations, where the major
concern would be separating contact induced convergence from inheritance
and/or more universal tendencies. However we also welcome contributions
dealing with detailed studies of two languages in contact (e.g. if they
look attentively at a particular lexical field and show how it is
organized), especially if these are situated within a broader linguistic
context, such as comparison with other genetically related languages,
and/or relate to findings in lexical-typological research.
References
Ameka, F. & D. Wilkins 1996. Semantics. In Goebl, H., P. Nelde, Z. Starý
& W. Wölck (eds.), /Contact linguistics/ (HSK). Berlin / New York:
Walter de Gruyter, 130-138.
Enfield, N. 2011, Taste in two tongues: a Southeast Asian study of
semantic convergence. In Majid, A. & S. Levinson (eds.), The senses in
language and culture, a spec. issue of /The senses & Society/, 06, 01,
30--37.
François, Alexandre. 2011. Social ecology and language history in the
northern Vanuatu linkage: A tale of divergence and convergence. /Journal
of Historical Linguistics/ 1 (2), 175-246. Nov 2011.
Haspelmath, M. & U. Tadmor (eds.) 2009. /Loanwords in the World's
Languages: A Comparative Handbook. /Berlin://Mouton de Gruyter.
Hayward Richard J. 2000. Is There a Metric for Convergence." In Renfrew,
C., A. McMahon and L. Trask (eds.), /Time Depth in Historical
Linguistics Vol 2 (Papers in the Prehistory of Languages)/, 621-640.
Cambrdige: The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
Hayward, Richard J. 1991. A propos patterns of lexicalization in the
Ethiopian Language Area. In Mendel, D. and U. Claudi (eds.), /Ägypten im
afroorientalischen Kontext./ /Special issue of Afrikanistische
Arbeitspapiere./ Cologne: Institute of African Studies, pp. 139-56.
Koptjevskaja-Tamm, M. 2011. Linguistic typology and language contact. In
Song, J.J. (ed.), /The Oxford/ /Handbook of Linguistic Typology/.
Oxford: OUP, 568--590.
Matisoff, James A. 2004. Areal semantics: is there such a thing? In:
Saxena, A. (ed.): Himalayan languages, past and present, 347-393.
Berlin: De Gruyter.
Matras, Y., and Sakel, J. 2007. 'Investigating the mechanisms of pattern
replication in language convergence', /Studies in Language, /31, 829--865/./
Treis, Yvonne. 2010. Perception verbs and taste adjectives in Kambaata
and beyond. In Anne Storch, (ed.), /Perception of the Invisible.
Religion, Historical Semantics and the Role of Perceptive Verbs/ (SUGIA
- Sprache und Geschichte in Afrika, 21) Cologne: Köppe. Pp. 313-346.
Urban, Matthias 2012. /Analyzability and semantic associations in
referring expressions/. PhD diss., Leiden university.
Vanhove, M. (ed.). 2008. /From polysemy to semantic change/. Amsterdam &
Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Zalizniak, A., M. Bulakh, D. Ganenkov, I. Gruntov, T. Maisak & M. Russo
2012. The catalogue of semantic shifts as a database for lexical
semantic typology. In Koptjevskaja-Tamm, M. & M. Vanhove (eds.), /New
directions in lexical/ /typology/. A special issue of /Linguistics, 50,
3/: 633 -- 669.
GUIDELINES FOR SUBMISSION
- For all abstracts the deadline of submission is January 15th 2013.
Authors are requested to register and upload their abstract individually
via the conference website www.sle2013.eu
<http://www.sle2013.eu>. Please mention the name of the workshop (or
some parts of it) next to the title of your paper.
- The abstracts should be (1) anonymous and (2) contain between 400 and
500 words (exclusive of references). (3) They should state research
questions, approach, method, data and (expected) results;
- The abstracts will receive three scores, two by two members of the SLE
2013 scientific committee and one by the workshop convenor(s).
- Workshop papers and general session papers will be selected according
to the same treshold. Last year the treshold was the average score of
all abstracts.
- Participants are allowed to present only one single-authored paper at
SLE 2013. In addition they may either have a joint paper (without being
the first author) or be a discussant in a workshop. Two co-authored
papers are also allowed.
- Workshop slots last 30 minutes. Slots may be combined to have a
session of an hour (cf. keynote speakers). The general schedule of the
conference may not be altered, however. For example, no slots of 20 or
40 minutes are allowed; no workshop will continue during the plenary
lectures or the poster session.
- Being part of the programme will be reserved for SLE members. Those
colleagues who are not a member yet, will be requested to become one at
the moment of registration (which will open in May 2013).
--
Maria Koptjevskaja Tamm
Office: Dept. of linguistics, Stockholm university, 106 91, Stockholm, Sweden
Home: Västerled 166, 167 72, Bromma, Sweden
tamm at ling.su.se,http://www.ling.su.se/tamm
tel.: +46-8-16 26 20 (office), +46-8-26 90 91 (home)
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://listserv.linguistlist.org/pipermail/lingtyp/attachments/20121218/7d1c9728/attachment.htm>
More information about the Lingtyp
mailing list