[Lingtyp] query: instrument voice

Mark Donohue mhdonohue at gmail.com
Tue Feb 22 03:24:22 UTC 2022


Hi David,

The issue, as I see it, is what we mean by 'promote'.
We can agree that

passive promotes object to subject (and demotes initial subject to non-core)
applicative promotes (oblique?) to object (might demote initial object to
non-core)

(and the philippine voice is something like " … promotes (anything) to
subject (and doesn't demote initial subject to non-core)

However, every text study of either passives or applicatives, or non-core
philippine-type voice choice, shows that there is a degree of pragmatic
prominence associated with the use of these valency-rearranging operations.
We might re-phrase the passive and applicative characterisations as

passive promotes pragmatically-prominent object to subject (and demotes
initial less-prominent subject to non-core)
applicative promotes pragmatically-prominent (oblique?) to object (might
demote initial (less-prominent?) object to non-core)

We typically describe applicatives as involving just the grammatical
function change. Thus, we have examples like this cited for Indonesian
(from Shiohara 2012):


   1.

   (2)a  Pelayan mengambil segelas air.

   waiter AV.take a.glass.of water

   ‘The waiter took a glass of water.’
   2.

   (2)b  Pelayan mengambil-kan tamu segelas air.
   waiter AV.take-APPL guest a.glass.of water
   ‘The waiter brought the guest a glass of water.’ (Sneddon 1996: 80)

As Susanna Cummings showed, however, (2)b examples are not really attested
in naturalistic discourse; rather, we have examples like the following:

(2)c  Tamu di-ambil-kan segelas air (oleh pelayan).
         guest NONACTIVE-take-APPL a.glass.of water by waiter
         'The waiter brought the guest a glass of water.'

(See also Donohue 2001 for similar data from Tukang Besi.)

So, this shows that (in some languages) the increased prominence of the
argument that was sufficient to merit coding with an applicative
construction is also sufficient to merit a non-active voice choice, with
all that entails. A Philippine-type voice system by stealth, as it were.

The Tzutujil example has an applicative suffix; and it also has a verb with
3SG absolutive agreement (Ø), not 1SG (the in- in the first example I
posted). It also has the requirement that there must be overt coding of the
increased prominent of the instrument; like Indonesian, it does that by
utilising existing high-prominence coding strategies; unlike Indonesian, it
does that not by using a voice change, but by using a pragmatically-marked
word order choice.

-Mark

Donohue, Mark. 2001. Coding choices in argument structure: Austronesian
applicatives in texts. *Studies in Language*25 (2): 217-254.






On Tue, 22 Feb 2022 at 13:53, David Gil <gil at shh.mpg.de> wrote:

> Mark,
>
> Thanks for the Tzutujil example, which is indeed quite similar to the New
> Guinea constructions I have been looking at.
>
> However, I remain unconvinced with regard to its characterization as an
> applicative, though to a certain degree this is a mere terminological
> question.  Prototypically, applicatives promote to direct objects while
> passive voices (such as instrumental) promote to subjects — so, for any
> given construction, the question is whether the relevant argument, here the
> instrumental one, is more direct-object-like or more subject-like.
>
> This begins to remind me of the seemingly endless ongoing debates over
> whether Philippine voice constructions are "really" passives or perhaps
> something else, the question generally boiling down to whether the relevant
> argument is more like a subject or more like a topic.  Personally, I don't
> find these debates very productive, and I'm not sure how useful an
> analogous debate between applicative and instrumental-voice labels would
> prove to be in this case.
>
> What's important is to have a clear description of the facts, and how the
> constructions in question differ from prototypical applicatives and from
> prototypical instrumental voice constructions — with the proviso that there
> are perhaps not sufficiently many of the latter to construct a clear notion
> of what is prototypical.
>
> David
>
>
> On 22/02/2022 04:26, Mark Donohue wrote:
>
> I would agree with Matthew that these are best described as applicatives,
> but ones in which the 'pragmatic advancement' function monitored by an
> applicative is, in addition to the grammatical function coding changes,
> also required to be monitored by the use of a pragmatically marked word
> order.
>
> Very similar facts are found in Tzutujil, in which the applicative, which
> indicates an instrumental role (despite having a morpheme cognate with the
> benefactive applicative in other Mayan languages) also requires the
> appearance of the instrument object in a preverbal role, which is a
> pragmatically marked position in a verb-initial language.
>
> Data from Dayley (1985).
>
> Xinruuch’eyi jaa7 tza7n chee7
>
> he:hit:me       he    with   stick
>
> ‘He hit me with a stick.’
>
> Chee7 x(r)uuch’eyb’ei jaa7 inin
>
> stick    he:hit-with:it     he    1SG
>
> ‘He hit me with a stick.’
>
> -Mark
>
> On Tue, 22 Feb 2022 at 13:15, David Gil <gil at shh.mpg.de> wrote:
>
>> Matthew,
>>
>> The reason I don't call it an applicative is that (in most cases) the
>> instrumental argument must occur before the verb in a topic-like position.
>>
>> This can be illustrated with the Roon instrumental prefix *u-* in the
>> following examples:
>>
>> (1)
>>
>> * Eros-i
>>
>> t-u-karuk
>>
>> ai-i-ya
>>
>>
>>
>> Eros-pers
>>
>> 3sg:anim-instr-chop
>>
>> tree-3sg:anim-def
>>
>>                   'Eros chopped the tree'
>>
>>
>>
>> (2)
>>
>> I-seref
>>
>> kaman
>>
>> fa
>>
>> Eros-i
>>
>> t-u-karuk
>>
>> ai-i-ya
>>
>>
>>
>> 1sg-look.for
>>
>> axe
>>
>> for
>>
>> Eros-pers
>>
>> 3sg:anim-instr-chop
>>
>> tree-3sg:anim-def
>>
>>                   'I'm looking for an axe for Eros to chop the tree with'
>>
>>
>>
>> (3)
>>
>> I-seref
>>
>> kaman
>>
>> Eros-i
>>
>> t-u-karuk
>>
>> ai-i-ya-ri-ya
>>
>>
>>
>> 1sg-look.for
>>
>> axe
>>
>> Eros-pers
>>
>> 3sg:anim-instr-chop
>>
>> tree-3sg:anim-def-3sg:inan-def
>>
>>                   'I'm looking for the axe that Eros chopped the tree
>> with'
>>
>> Sentence (1) is ungrammatical, and cannot be salvaged by adding a
>> postverbal NP or PP referring to the axe; in this respect it differs from
>> typical applicative constructions.  In contrast, sentences (2) and (3) are
>> fine, because the instrumental prefix *u-* is licensed by the preceding
>> NP *kaman* referring to the axe.  True, this is not exactly the same as
>> how things work in Philippine languages, but it is more like Philippine
>> instrumental voice than anything else I can think of (including
>> applicatives).  In particular, in (3), the instrumental prefix is required
>> in order to license relativization (in contrast, relativization of other
>> oblique arguments is zero-marked).  To use Paul Schachter's terminology, in
>> both (2) and (3), "subjecthood properties" seem to be split between the
>> agent (which, as you correctly point out, controls agreement) and the
>> instrument.
>>
>> Very similar patterns obtain in the other Austronesian and
>> non-Austronesian languages that I mentioned, which — given the apparent
>> rarity of this pattern elsewhere — is strongly suggestive of language
>> contact.
>>
>> David
>>
>>
>> On 22/02/2022 03:41, Matthew Dryer wrote:
>>
>> David,
>>
>>
>>
>> Why would you not say that the instrumental construction in Meyah, Sougb,
>> and Hatam is an applicative, since the A rather than the instrument
>> controls subject agreement?
>>
>>
>>
>> Matthew
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
>> *From: *Lingtyp <lingtyp-bounces at listserv.linguistlist.org>
>> <lingtyp-bounces at listserv.linguistlist.org> on behalf of David Gil
>> <gil at shh.mpg.de> <gil at shh.mpg.de>
>> *Date: *Monday, February 21, 2022 at 7:40 PM
>> *To: *"lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org"
>> <lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org> <lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org>
>> <lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org>
>> *Subject: *[Lingtyp] query: instrument voice
>>
>>
>>
>> Dear all,
>>
>>
>>
>> In the Austronesian languages of Taiwan, Philippines and Madagascar,
>> there is a verbal affix that is said to mark "instrument voice"; loosely
>> speaking, it marks the topic or subject of the clause as bearing the
>> semantic role of instrument.
>>
>>
>>
>> Is anybody familiar with similar instrument-voice constructions from
>> other parts of the world?
>>
>>
>>
>> The reason I ask is that a similar construction is present also in some
>> languages of the Bird's Head and Cenderawasih Bay regions of New Guinea,
>> eg. Biak, Roon, Wamesa and Wooi (Austronesian), and Hatam, Sougb, Meyah and
>> Moskona (non-Austronesian).  What's curious about this construction is
>> that, unlike the well-known Austronesian cases, it is the only
>> morphologically-marked voice in each of the languages in question; there is
>> no "ordinary" morphological passive construction.  My feeling is that this
>> construction is quite uncommon cross-linguistically, but I would like to
>> get a feel for the extent to which this is indeed true.
>>
>>
>>
>> Thanks,
>>
>>
>>
>> David
>>
>> --
>>
>> David Gil
>>
>>
>>
>> Senior Scientist (Associate)
>>
>> Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution
>>
>> Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
>>
>> Deutscher Platz 6, Leipzig, 04103, Germany
>>
>>
>>
>> Email: gil at shh.mpg.de
>>
>> Mobile Phone (Israel): +972-526117713
>>
>> Mobile Phone (Indonesia): +62-81344082091
>>
>>
>>
>> --
>> David Gil
>>
>> Senior Scientist (Associate)
>> Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution
>> Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
>> Deutscher Platz 6, Leipzig, 04103, Germany
>>
>> Email: gil at shh.mpg.de
>> Mobile Phone (Israel): +972-526117713
>> Mobile Phone (Indonesia): +62-81344082091
>>
>>
>> _______________________________________________
>> Lingtyp mailing list
>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>
> --
> David Gil
>
> Senior Scientist (Associate)
> Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution
> Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
> Deutscher Platz 6, Leipzig, 04103, Germany
>
> Email: gil at shh.mpg.de
> Mobile Phone (Israel): +972-526117713
> Mobile Phone (Indonesia): +62-81344082091
>
>
>
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://listserv.linguistlist.org/pipermail/lingtyp/attachments/20220222/4cc67ce1/attachment.htm>


More information about the Lingtyp mailing list