signed phonemes, iconicity etc.

Sonja Erlenkamp sonja.erlenkamp at hist.no
Tue Oct 2 16:15:55 UTC 2007


Sorry all, 
 
regarding my last e-mail: I intended to use the subject Dorothea Cogill-Koez suggested but forgot to change it before hitting the send button... :(
 
Sonja

________________________________

Fra: slling-l-bounces at majordomo.valenciacc.edu på vegne av Sonja Erlenkamp
Sendt: ti 02.10.2007 17:57
Til: A list for linguists interested in signed languages
Emne: SV: SV: [SLLING-L] use of sign language in Jordan


Ok, I'll try to give a short answer for now (both due to time constrains on my side and the wish to not loose all readers of this discussion).
 
1. I agree that iconicity is a mental process. Nevertheless are there a structural markers to trigger it. You mentioned that there is "something" you see. This something is very often eye gaze in the signed languages I have seen and analyzed and some other markers, some of them probably different from signed language to another (I have found some markers for Norwegian Sign Language for triggering "real space blend entities". )
 
2. I don't agree on the part that iconicity is only "etymological" and that it can be "re-perceived" as depicting. I think that signed languages make use of two differend (cognitive) processes/mechanisms. On the one hand we have iconicity as part of the creation process of signs. Many of these signs develop after a while - through entrechment - a fixed semantic - that's where we agree. Although these signs still might have some kind of transparent iconic look (not all of them have) the iconicity does not play any role in the meaning construction in a signed text or the decoding of the meaning of the particular sign and why not use the phonemes concept on them? I have no problems with that.
 
In addition I think (that is where we disagree), that we also have signs and signed construction that need their iconic potential to trigger real space blend entities in order to construct meaning. They are not "re-preceived" as depiciting signs, they are actually different signs which are depicting to begin with. 
 
 
3. Both mechanisms described above go hand in hand in a signed language and make it a visually efficient communication system. And if you take a look at the behaviour of for example mouthing, sign-modifications and  word order you'll see that it follows the two different mechanisms. And I don't think either of these mechanisms is going to disappear in the future, because they are the pillars of visual communication.
And that is why I think the phoneme-system model is not sufficient. It is not entirely wrong, but it leaves us with descriptions of signs within one of these mechanisms, but gives no answer to the other. I am not against using phonemes for spoken or signed languages (actually I studied spoken language phonology and I loved it!), but I think we need a wider perspective to cover everything that is going in signed language communication.
 
All the best
 
Sonja Erlenkamp
 

________________________________

Fra: slling-l-bounces at majordomo.valenciacc.edu på vegne av dcogill at une.edu.au
Sendt: ti 02.10.2007 14:09
Til: A list for linguists interested in signed languages
Emne: Re: SV: [SLLING-L] use of sign language in Jordan



>Sonja wrote:

Part A....
> Personally I believe that many, if not allmost of all the parameters in
a
> single sign and including nonmanual features can (and often do) carry an
iconic potential which makes them by definition non-arbitrary

Part B...
> and that
means again they could > not be phonemic in the sense of spoken language
phonemes, because phonemes are
> by definition arbitrary.


I absolutely agree with Part A here - in fact I'd remove the 'almost' and
say "....all the parameters in a single sign" do actually carry an iconic
*potential*, at least - it's simply that in routine use that iconicity is
not exploited or even consciously perceived. When a teenage signer
discussing his school activities can even smoothly convert an ASL letter
sequence like "TC" (for Total Commmunication) into a mime of smoking and
drinking (oh dear - does anyone have the reference for that?) - then it's
quite hopeless to try to identify and enumerate some kind of finite iconic
potential in the component forms of each visible sign itself.

But doesn't the work of the 1970s, summarised in Klima and Bellugi's work
(1979), show us that we shouldn't expect the level of visible form to hold
the answers as to a sign's 'iconicity'?  I would say that their work has
shown that one must focus on the level of cognitive processing. It's at
THIS level - the level of a signer's immediate, online intentions and
perceptions - that a sign 'is' an abstract symbol, or a manual depiction.

So practically every time an ASL signer forms the ASL alphabet letters T
and D it's processed as a simple, abstract symbol with a direct,
unmediated cognitive link to its meaning - I'm sure we'd all agree.  In
fact K&B's work on sign assimilation processes, historical drift patterns
etc, and the subsequent work on aphasias by Poizner and colleagues and by
others since then, have shown that regular lexical signs, whether
obviously potentially iconic or not, are at least *routinely* produced and
perceived online (that is, during actual conversation) as if they were
simple abstract counters directly triggering meaning.

Yet I guess it's equally agreed that the very SAME forms can suddenly be
're-perceived' as manual pictures, when a signer is so inclined.  The key
argument here is 're-conceived' - it's primarily a different *cognitive*
process that's going on when this happens. Naturally, then, the sign can
be physically morphed and manipulated in a way that no longer follows the
formational rules of that sign, but this is a natural SECONDARY
consequence of the re-conceptualisation. The real, important thing (I'd
argue) is the switch in perception and cognitive processsing.

Yes, of course, the signer then must attach some signals to convey to
their interlocutor "put on your picture-perceiving glasses right now and
actively process this as a manual depiction, related to the immediate
context, including the regular lexical meaning of the original sign I'm
re-conceptualising"'.  In Auslan CPs, which I argue with tedious
persistence are ALWAYS manual depictions (just of a level of structural
sophistication and conventionalisation that speech-based iconic gesure
never reaches, and hence unrecognised by a speech-based model of
communication) - in Auslan CPs the switch, according to Trevor Johnston,
can be signalled by a pause - I also think (and Trevor Johnston may have
also said...?) that Auslan signers tend to look AT their hands in these CP
manual depictions, as if to say "this is something to be 'seen', not just
immediately and unthinkingly linked to its meaning as a regular sign can
be", and also that signers tend to look back up at the end of a CP
depiction as if to say "see that? Got it?" - because after all depictions,
however structured and convention-governed, are much less predictable in
form than regular lexical signs that have citation forms.

But what happens to signal to the interlocutor that a *regular lexical
sign* is to be re-conceptualised as a picture? A slowing of the sign?  A
knowing look? Extra body involvement?  What do people notice? I know
there's something, since even I can often pick 'this guy's switched to
using the sign as a depiction not a symbol'. But what?.....

Anyway, the point of all this (I assure you there is one....:-))  is that
if one models the relationship of 'iconicity' and lexical sign in  this
way, then
1. one gets rid of "if there is iconicity, then there cannot be phonemes
in signed languages"; the Part B of your statement above, which which I
strenuously disagree - or at least, that I think it is logically
unneccessary to agree with! ;-)
2. more generally, one can accommodate the fact that the bulk of a system
like a signed communication, so rich in superficial iconicity in its
forms, can nevertheless be genuinely governed, at the online cognitive
level, by all the same abstract grammatical structures as govern the most
complex spoken language grammar known to man (or woman);
2. one can perhaps, I think, solve the notation problem posed by 'iconicity'.

As regards the first issue, "if there is iconicity. there can be no
phonemes" - I'd say this is just a mistake arising from focussing on
iconicity as a surface phenomenon, and conflating that with what "must" be
happening at the deeper cognitive level. If the underlying ONLINE
PROCESSING of that sign is via the direct, abstract link to meaning,
unmediated by online perception of the *picture* in the sign, then surface
iconicity (in such cases) is a red herring - it doesn't matter at all!
No?

This was Klima and Bellugi's - and Poizner's - lesson for us all, after
all - old hat to us; at that moment of online use, for signers, a regular
lexical form is routinely perceived as a collocation of formal structural
elements that map onto a mental representation stored as a ...well, as
whatever the phonemes of spoken languages are cognitively and neurally
stored as. (Hence damage to the classic language areas, which doesn't
affect picture perception, knocks out signed communication via regular
lexical signs and grammar, but damage to the right hemisphere that affects
picture perception and CPs doesn't affect regular lexcial sign, etc etc.)
At a cognitive and neural level, THIS USE of regular lexical sign is as
'phonological' as you please! It may not involve 'phones' in the sense of
sound - but who cares? :-) - as long as we know we're claiming to be
talking about the
underlying, asbtract, formal elements of linguistic tokens (signed or
spoken) which are processed via a direct online link to meaning.

Moving on to the second point; for the same reason, freed of the online
control of the active principle of depiction, superficially iconic signs
can of course go on to be governed in their online behaviour (routinely)
by an abstract grammatical forces one might wish to impute, from
principles and parameters (or whatever it cuurrently is) to
historically-iconically-derived but now cognitively independent
grammatical principles, such as most cognitive linguists believe govern
spoken systems.

The third point is that this model frees one of the need to record
'iconicity' or 'iconic potential' as an abstract, enduring property of
each sign itself.  It seems to me that it might allow us, rather, to
simply use a bracketing or superscript line or somesuch, to show that the
forms so marked have, in THIS CASE of use, just here been re-conceived, by
these signers, as manual pictures.

Well, a superscript would arguably be a good strategy at the 'phonemic'
level of sign writing!  Or more accurately, since the term 'phonemic'
really does become a misnomer here - that would be, arguably, a solution
at the level of 'recording the cognitive construct  through which a sign
is constructed and decoded'.

At the ...oh, and now 'phonetic' is probably a misnomer too, albeit less
badly!  At the SR level of 'recording the actual visual forms produced',
SR might record, rather, the *physical* signals that signal to the
interlocutor "put on your picture-perceiving glasses"; the details of gaze
change, facial expression, body involvement, rhythm change, whatever they
turn out to be in a given signed communication system or a given
individual case.

Still, either way, it's a whole lot simpler than trying to pin down
'iconicity' in the superficial form of each and every sign itself- surely
a quite impossible task, as well as a fundamentally unnecessary one.  It
would be moving the focus to the flip-side of Klima and Bellugi's 1979
paradigm-setting work: it would be moving to defining the PICTURES in
signs, as well as the SYMBOLS in signs, as primarily having being, not in
the form iself, but in the minds producing and perceiving the forms.

Well, I was going to post something else discussing a point that Dan
Parvaz raised, but I'm a wee bit embarrassed by the length of this post.
I'm afraid I'm inclined to rave on a bit when it gets to my pet topic of
the cognitive processing of signed communication systems. I do have a book
in preparation that includes these arguments, so I wish I could say it's
all in Cogill-Koez forthcoming, but with my workload at the moment I'm
wondering if it the book will ever 'forthcome'!

Dorothea.

Dr Dorothea Cogill-Koez,
Language and Cognition Research Centre
School of Behavioural, Cognitive and Social Sciences,
University of New England,
Armidale, NSW,
AUSTRALIA

>
>
>
> ________________________________
>
> Connect to the next generation of MSN Messenger  Get it now!
> <http://imagine-msn.com/messenger/launch80/default.aspx?locale=en-us&source=wlmailtagline>
_______________________________________________
> SLLING-L mailing list
> SLLING-L at majordomo.valenciacc.edu
> http://majordomo.valenciacc.edu/mailman/listinfo/slling-l
>











_______________________________________________
SLLING-L mailing list
SLLING-L at majordomo.valenciacc.edu
http://majordomo.valenciacc.edu/mailman/listinfo/slling-l



-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://listserv.linguistlist.org/pipermail/slling-l/attachments/20071002/30bd0b2f/attachment.htm>
-------------- next part --------------
_______________________________________________
SLLING-L mailing list
SLLING-L at majordomo.valenciacc.edu
http://majordomo.valenciacc.edu/mailman/listinfo/slling-l


More information about the Slling-l mailing list