[Lingtyp] Morphologically complex clitics?
Larry M. HYMAN
hyman at berkeley.edu
Tue Apr 6 15:32:13 UTC 2021
Dear Adam,
I am responding for several reasons. First, I personally did not find your
expanding (escalating) the discussion to be particularly useful in
identifying the kinds of phenomena that Florian had clearly delineated (as
can be seen from the several LINGTYP responses, as well as private ones he
received). To say that essentially everything "leans" is not helpful at
all, as if we cannot find independent phonological criteria for elements
that have clitic-like behavior. If a framework prefers to reanalyze these
(or even outlaw them), this also seems besides the point which was to
identify phenomena at the descriptive level, where there is already enough
to disentangle. I want to thank Florian for his inquiry, which we can take
as an invitation to identify examples where morphologically complex clitics
appear to exist and determine what their properties are.
For example, you hypothesize that the Luganda example might (instead) be
treated as a string of clitics. However, considering the total facts of
Luganda one would better recognize that =byange 'my' begins with the same
noun class 8 agreement prefix bi- found in other word classes, e.g.
(e-)bi-tabo 'books', bi-rúngi 'good', bi-biri 'two', bi-rî 'those (far)'
etc.). I also indicated a phonological criterion for clitichood: the
preservation of final vowel length of the preceding syllable: (e-)bi-d*éé*
=byange 'my bells' vs. (e)bi-d*e* bi-biri 'two bells'. Of course the total
picture is more complex than this, as Francis and I reported in
excruciating detail (Hyman & Katamba 1990, "Final vowel shortening in
Luganda", *Studies in African Linguistics*). But there is good reason to
assert that class 8 =by-ange 'my', =by-o 'your sg.' and =by-e 'his/her' are
(i) single clitics and (ii) morphologically complex. Besides preserving
preceding length, they cannot stand alone. Thus, compare these with the
corresponding self-standing independent possessive pronouns: e-by-áange
'mine', e-bí-by-ô 'yours sg.', and e-bí-by-ê 'his/hers', which are fuller
and have different tone properties.
Best, Larry
On Mon, Apr 5, 2021 at 4:20 AM Adam James Ross Tallman <
ajrtallman at utexas.edu> wrote:
> Dear Florian *inter alia,*
>
> It's never really clear to me whether with questions like these, we are
> asking:
>
> 1. Are there descriptions which posit such things (i.e. use a
> terminological framework that allow them to "exist" and then describe them
> as "existing")?
>
> 2. Is there a thing like this in the language (regardless of the
> terminological framework because all of the relevant notions have obvious
> cross-linguistically applicable diagnostics / criteria)?
>
> For 1, note that for some authors (like Stephen Anderson, circa 2005)
> morphologically complex clitics seem to be logically impossible. Clitics
> are either syntactic elements that integrate into (post-lexical)
> phonological structure (simple/phonological clitics) or else they are
> morphological elements themselves that operate over phrases. Since they are
> supposed to be realizational elements themselves, it's hard to even imagine
> a morphologically complex clitic being something that's allowed.
>
> *Is Anderson making an empirical claim about the absence of
> morphologically complex clitics cross-linguistically?* I don't think so.
> They are just ruled out on terminological / theoretical grounds. I assume
> the Luganda case would just be analyzed as a string of clitics - where
> their place in the grammar would be (as post-lexical phonological
> integration or post-lexical /phrase level placement or both) determined,
> would then depend on what you meant by "normal syntax".
>
> In terms of 'phonologically leaning' ... according to which phonological
> process (if any)? You can always vacuously phonologically lean into an
> utterance/intonational phrase. In much prosodic phonology, an element can
> be categorized,tautologically as phonologically leaning just because it
> surfaces at all (it would integrate into an intonational phrase (that need
> only be identified by pause breaks), or an empirically vacuous layer of the
> prosodic hierarchy). So for some linguists, this just isn't a meaningful
> empirical question at all. Not that it's not interesting, we just have to
> know a lot more about how one goes about establishing the bridging
> principles between notions like g-word and p-word and the actual language
> data.
>
> If the question is - *can *such structural classifications exist. Well,
> yeah. You just have to choose definitions of '=' and '-' such that a
> morphologically complex clitic falls out. For instance, in Chacobo there's
> this punctual morpheme *=/-tápi. *It allows some variable ordering with
> "suffixes" and "clitics" in the verb complex - it could be categorized as
> either an affix or clitic depending on what criteria you use. When the verb
> complex occurs with an associated motion morpheme, *=/-tapi *happens to
> have a fixed position after the AM morpheme and it happens to modify the
> motion action. *SO*, just choose a definition of g-word that makes *-tapi
> *an affix and the AM morphemes simple clitics. And voilà, you have a
> morphologically complex clitic. Is this an example of a morphologically
> complex clitic? If you want it to be, yes.
>
> Thus, it's hard to know how 2 is being addressed in any of the
> discussions, because '=' and '-' are not defined. So implicitly people are
> responding to 1. Which is fine, it's always informative to know how much
> terminological variation there is in the field, but it's harder to
> interpret what it all means.
>
> best,
>
> Adam
>
>
>
>
> On Wed, Mar 31, 2021 at 10:14 PM Martin Haspelmath <
> martin_haspelmath at eva.mpg.de> wrote:
>
>> But what exactly is "a clitic word"?
>>
>> In Luganda, it may be uncontroversial that there are two words in
>> *e-bi-déé=by-a-nge* 'my cups', because *by-aa-* also occurs as a
>> proclitic elsewhere.
>>
>> But in Quechua *wasi-bi-chu-ga-n* 'is not at home', how do we know that
>> there is a "clitic word" *-chu* and a "clitic word" *-ga-n*, rather than
>> three clitics *=chu*, *=ga*, and *=n*? Is this because *-ga-* looks like
>> a "verb stem", and we have the idea that verbs are "inflected"?
>>
>> Since Schiering et al. (2010) (doi:10.1017/S0022226710000216), it has
>> been widely known that "p-(rosodic) word" is not a generally applicable
>> notion, which casts even more doubt on the notion of "clitic word".
>>
>> But if we consider items that are traditionally considered "clitics" in
>> European languages, it's really easy to find complex ones among the bound
>> person forms, e.g. French *l-e/l-a/l-es*, Italian *m-i/t-i/s-i*, Greek
>> *to-n/ti-n/tu-s/t-u/ti-s*, Bulgarian *n-i/v-i/g-i*.
>>
>> Martin
>>
>> Am 31.03.21 um 05:56 schrieb Larry M. HYMAN:
>>
>> Hi Florian,
>>
>> I was expecting lots of offers over the past 16 hours, but none! In Bantu
>> this is quite usual because clitics often combine noun class agreement with
>> whatever the marker is--often fused. E.g. in Luganda the locative enclitic
>> =kô 'on it, a little' consists of class 17 ku- and -o. The "connective"
>> (associative, genitive) prefixes the noun class agreement to /-a/ (ebitabo
>> byaa=Walúsimbi 'Walusimbi's books', from class 8 /bi-a/), and so forth.
>> Several of the possessive pronoun enclitics are bisyllabic, e.g. class 8
>> byange = /bi-a-nge/ 'my', as in e-bi-déé =by-a-nge 'my cups' (where the
>> enclitic saves the length of the root -déé 'bell(s)' from undergoing final
>> vowel shortening.
>>
>> There are lots of such examples in the following paper:
>>
>> Hyman, Larry M. & Francis X. Katamba. 1990. Final vowel shortening in
>> Luganda. *Studies in African Linguistics *21.1-59, available here:
>>
>> https://journals.flvc.org/sal/article/view/107438/102758
>>
>> Best, Larry
>>
>> On Tue, Mar 30, 2021 at 4:36 AM <florian.matter at isw.unibe.ch> wrote:
>>
>>> Dear all,
>>>
>>> I am looking for examples of morphologically complex clitics — i.e.,
>>> g-words that a) do not form their own p-words and b) consist of multiple
>>> morphemes. Below are some of the few examples I have found. In (1-2), it is
>>> an encliticized copula which carries person inflection. In (3), the verb
>>> complex consists of a finite verb, a converb, and an auxiliary, each their
>>> own g-word. Both the finite verb and the auxiliary are inflected for first
>>> person and therefore morphologically complex.
>>>
>>> (1) Trió (Cariban)
>>> əmamina-nə=pəə*=w-a-e*
>>> play-INF=occ.with=1Sa-be-NPST.CERT
>>> 'I am playing' (Meira 1999: 180)
>>>
>>> (2) Ecuadorian Quechua
>>> paj mana wasi-bi=t͡ʃu*=ga-n*
>>> 3PRO NEG house-LOC=NEG=be-3
>>> 'S/he is not at home.' (Muysken 2010: 197)
>>>
>>> (3) Nangikurrunggurr (Southern Daly)
>>> jawul karicinmade *ŋebem=*wuɹic*=ŋiɹim* catma
>>> spear bent 1SG.S.bash.PRS=fix=1SG.S.sit.PRS straight
>>> 'I'm sitting straightening this bent spear.' (Reid 2003: 114)
>>>
>>> I am grateful for any further examples of such patterns, or references
>>> to literature on morphologically complex clitics.
>>>
>>> Best,
>>> Florian
>>>
>>> _____________________________
>>>
>>> Universität Bern
>>>
>>> Institut für Sprachwissenschaft
>>>
>>> Florian Matter
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>> Länggassstrasse 49
>>>
>>> CH-3012 Bern
>>>
>>> Tel. +41 31 631 37 54
>>>
>>> Raum B 168
>>>
>>> *florian.matter at isw.unibe.ch <florian.matter at isw.unibe.ch>*
>>>
>>> *http://www.isw.unibe.ch <http://www.isw.unibe.ch/>*
>>> _______________________________________________
>>> Lingtyp mailing list
>>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
>>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>>
>>
>>
>> --
>> Larry M. Hyman, Professor of Linguistics & Executive Director,
>> France-Berkeley Fund
>> Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley
>> https://linguistics.berkeley.edu/~hyman
>>
>> _______________________________________________
>> Lingtyp mailing listLingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.orghttp://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>
>>
>> --
>> Martin Haspelmath
>> Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
>> Deutscher Platz 6
>> D-04103 Leipzighttps://www.shh.mpg.de/employees/42385/25522
>>
>> _______________________________________________
>> Lingtyp mailing list
>> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
>> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>>
>
>
> --
> Adam J.R. Tallman
> Post-doctoral Researcher
> Friedrich Schiller Universität
> Department of English Studies
> _______________________________________________
> Lingtyp mailing list
> Lingtyp at listserv.linguistlist.org
> http://listserv.linguistlist.org/mailman/listinfo/lingtyp
>
--
Larry M. Hyman, Professor of Linguistics & Executive Director,
France-Berkeley Fund
Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley
https://linguistics.berkeley.edu/~hyman
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://listserv.linguistlist.org/pipermail/lingtyp/attachments/20210406/1a00f2cf/attachment.htm>
More information about the Lingtyp
mailing list